Skip to main content
  • Other Publications
  • Subscribe
  • Contact Us
Advertisement
JCORE Reference
this is the JCORE Reference site slogan
  • Home
  • Most Read
  • About Us
    • About Us
    • Editorial Board
  • More
    • Advertising
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
    • Folders
    • Help
  • Patients
  • Reference Site Links
    • View Regions
  • Archive

Theoretical Considerations for Chronic Pain Rehabilitation

Martin Lotze, G. Lorimer Moseley
DOI: 10.2522/ptj.20140581 Published 1 September 2015
Martin Lotze
M. Lotze, MD, Functioning Imaging Unit, University of Greifswald, W Rathenau Str 46, Greifswald, 17475 Germany.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
G. Lorimer Moseley
G.L. Moseley, PhD, Sansom Institute for Health Research, University of South Australia, Adelaide, Australia.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Conventional rehabilitation of patients with chronic pain is often not successful and is frustrating for the treatment team. However, theoretical developments and substantial advances in our understanding of the neurological aspects of chronic pain are changing these experiences. Modern theoretical models of pain consider pain to be a perceptual inference that reflects a “best guess” that protective action is required. This article argues that keen observation and open and respectful clinician-patient and scientist-clinician relationships have been critical for the emergence of effective rehabilitation approaches and will be critical for further improvements. The role in modern pain rehabilitation of reconceptualizing the pain itself—by “Explaining Pain,” careful and intentional observation of the person in pain, and the strategic and constant communication of safety—is emphasized. It also is suggested that better understanding of the neural mechanisms underpinning chronic pain has directly informed the development of new therapeutic approaches, which are being further refined and tested. Conventional pain treatment (where the clinician strives to find the pain-relieving medication or exercise) or pain management (where the clinician helps the patient to manage life despite unabating pain) is being replaced by pain rehabilitation, where a truly biopsychosocial approach allows clinicians to provide patients with the knowledge, understanding, and skills to reduce both their pain and disability. A brief overview is provided of the key aspects of modern pain rehabilitation and the considerations that should lead our interaction with patients with chronic pain.

Chronic pain exerts a massive burden on society.1 The landmark work of Fordyce2 triggered a shift away from the idea of pain treatments toward the idea of “management,” whereby clinicians—and, therefore, patients—aim to optimize quality of life despite unabating pain. Here, we outline the theoretical and practical considerations that underpin the re-emergence of pain rehabilitation and present examples of how scientific discoveries are being transformed into effective rehabilitation approaches. By so doing, we hope to not only help rehabilitation practitioners but also increase interaction between scientists and clinicians on the approaches and understanding of chronic pain. Scientists pursue answers to questions, but it is the clinicians, and the patients themselves, who identify the questions that need answering. Similarly, by engaging frankly and humbly with scientists, clinicians can draw on a very large community of people with the energy, skills, and resources to provide unbiased answers to their questions.

Old Lessons Ring True

The substantial increase in our appreciation of the complexity of pain biology has led to the emergence of a common implication: to first and foremost observe. Clinically, observation refers to careful appraisal of patients and their situation (ie, to ask questions and to listen carefully to the responses). To really listen is to focus your attention on the patients, on what the patients say (and, indeed, on what they do not say) and how they say it—not just the words they use, but the entire behavioral package (ie, their manner, their posture, their ease of articulation, their expression, and the attributions they provide for their pain). Recognition that every aspect of this behavioral package reflects activation of neural representations compels us to integrate those things into our clinical reasoning. In a truly biopsychosocial framework of pain, the things we say, do, think, and hear are all potential modulators of pain itself and may all be suitable targets for rehabilitation (for a review, see Butler and Moseley3). Clearly, then, the relationship we develop with the patient in pain is critical.

This critical relationship that exists between the clinician and the patient also is reflected in, and can direct and inform, that between the clinician and the scientist. Indeed, careful observation of the person in pain requires scientists to honor Darwin's advice to most carefully note those observations that do not support our current perspectives.4 Noting rather than disregarding such observations has opened up new fields of investigation in chronic pain rehabilitation. For example, that some patients are made worse by simply imagining movements of a painful limb5,6 or that some patients declare they have a swollen limb that is, in fact, not swollen7 presumably went unnoted for some time but has now been investigated with empirical methods, and new treatments have been generated as a result. What, then, should clinicians be observing? We think the answer to this question has been radically changed as a result of recent theoretical and neuroscientific developments in our field.

Pain as a Conscious Motivator to Protect Our Body

It should be remembered, as we grapple with the complexity of pain and its sometimes tenuous relationship with tissue damage, that pain exists only in our own consciousness. Pain might be considered a perceptual inference—a “best guess” that protective action is required. Not surprisingly, then, although pain is universally attributed to some mix of brain activity, exactly how the feeling emerges as a result of the brain activity remains part of what Chalmers called “the hard problem.”8 We avoid this problem clinically by focusing on causal and associative relationships we see and theoretical models, or “thinking stories” that we use to understand those relationships.

Pain is clearly related to both external (exogenous) and internal (endogenous) stimuli. These relationships are modulated by other sensory inputs and cognitive, emotional, and social factors that can be broadly encompassed by the concept of “context.” This sometimes extraordinary modulatory power (compare, for example, the pain evoked by identical cold stimuli in the presence of either a blue or red light9) is not unique to pain. Scientists investigate such modulations in far less emotionally charged conscious experiences (eg, vision). An overly simplistic understanding of vision would suggest that what we see is an accurate representation of the imprint of the pattern of light that is hitting the retina. However, one realizes this is not the case when one considers common visual illusions, where cues about the size of everyday objects, the patterns of light and shadow, and color cues—all informing about context—can fundamentally change what we see.

Think about the Necker cube experiments,10 experiments on color recognition using mondrians,11 and the McGurk illusion.12 Each of these tells us that perception, including but not limited to pain, reflects the brain's best guess as to what behavioral response is appropriate and what conscious experience will invoke it.13

Very important discoveries from the study of nociception continue to move forward our pursuit for better pharmacological pain therapies. However, the theoretical perspective of pain as a perceptual inference based on both nociceptive and non-nociceptive information has re-ignited the pain rehabilitation field. That nociception is neither sufficient nor necessary for pain14 and that pain is an experience that urges us to do something to protect a body part15 are not new ideas, but the potential influence of their recent endorsement cannot be overstated. The loosening grip of the structural-pathology model, in which pain was considered a readout of nociceptive input and a measure of tissue damage, has triggered this shift in our approach. It is an important shift because, as long as chronic pain was considered analogous to chronic nociception, only treatments that reduce nociception would make any sense. It is perhaps not surprising that the clinical impact of treatments for chronic pain that are based on this structural-pathology model has been underwhelming to say the least.

Clinical Implications

This idea of pain as a perceptual inference that urges protective action infers that pain will be modulated by anything that weights the inference toward protection. That is, any input—exogenous, endogenous, cognitive, social, or contextual—that provides “credible evidence of danger” to body tissue, as well as the current state of the organism,3 will increase the likelihood and intensity of pain. We can observe the patient through this lens of danger versus safety. What credible evidence is there of danger or safety? During the interview and the physical assessment, are there cues that alert the keenly observant clinician to modulations of perceived danger? Assessment expands from evaluation of tissue integrity and mechanical performance to evaluation of anything that implies danger or safety. Such an approach is captured in self-assessment and therapeutic tools such as the Protectometer.3

One exciting aspect of the advances in pain-related knowledge is that changing the meaning of pain—changing how an individual in pain makes sense of their own pain—should change pain itself.3 The therapeutic approach that directly targets this aspect, which has come to be known as “Explaining Pain,” refers to a range of educational interventions that aim to change someone's understanding of what pain actually is. Although “pain education” has been a part of cognitive-behavioral management programs for decades, that education was limited to the application of the science rather than the science itself—for example, teaching patients that pain does not equal harm; that you should move despite pain; and that pain is unavoidable, but suffering is optional.16

Explaining Pain covers the science itself. That is, it aims to provide an understanding of the biological rationale for doing those things. Explaining Pain is an approach to management rather than a specific set of procedures or techniques (and, critically, an approach that dovetails with cognitive-behavioral techniques). The core objective of Explaining Pain is to shift one's conceptualization of pain from that of a marker of tissue damage or pathology to that of a marker of the perceived need to protect body tissue. Explaining Pain has thus far taken several different formats—from intensive one-on-one, small-group, tutorial-type sessions to large-group seminars lasting up to 3 hours.17,18 The approach has been adapted according to preference and economics, and the material has been condensed19,20 or incorporated into booklets21 or storybooks.22 Systematic reviews concluded that Explaining Pain is probably effective for reducing pain and disability in people with a range of chronic pain states.23,24

The Dark Side of Plasticity

The implications of these developments extend beyond Explaining Pain. There is now a large body of literature on the differences in central nervous system function between people with chronic pain and healthy controls. These differences are thought to reflect, in part, “maladaptive neuroplasticity.”25 Put simply, the longer you have pain, the better your system gets at producing it; pain becomes triggered more easily (allodynia), and previously painful events become more painful (hyperalgesia). Chronic pain also is associated with abnormal intracortical inhibitory mechanisms26 whereby body-related neural representations become less precise, an abnormality thought to be important in some of the multiple system dysfunctions that are seen in people with chronic pain.27 That this increased sensitivity and decreased precision may contribute to the problem of pathological pain has generated promising new approaches to rehabilitation.

Retraining the Brain: Graded Motor Imagery

Graded motor imagery (GMI) begins with implicit motor imagery, where the patient views images of body parts (eg, hands) and makes a judgment about whether the body part that is displayed belongs to the left or the right side of the body. Graded motor imagery then progresses to explicit motor imagery, where the patient actively imagines adopting the posture shown in the image and then progresses to mirror box training.28 It has now been more than 10 years since the first randomized controlled trials of GMI were published. Although GMI was developed with a clear biological rationale based on established properties of neural activation with implicit and explicit motor imagery,29 data are now available for more than 450 patients who have completed the GMI protocol, about 20% of them having participated in randomized controlled trials.

Those data reveal some very interesting patterns. For example, the degree to which patients participate appears to be very important; results clearly drop when participants drop beneath 7 training sessions a day, each for several minutes. This is a clear limitation of the approach because that training load is substantial and the patient needs to remain very motivated. The patient might be motivated by feedback and increasing demand of the training process. The clinician can control the training time and development online so he or she can take action when participation is decreasing. The role of the clinician is rather as a coach than as an empathic doctor who is expected to relieve the patient's pain.2 Therefore, classical clinical role models do not apply here, and cognitive and behavioral principles are important. Indeed, although the available published and audit data are building a positive picture, the clinician-patient relationship is likely to be critical. For example, some clinical centers report much better outcomes than others, even though all are ostensibly doing the same thing, and the consistency of the conceptual paradigms among the treatment team seems to be important. The power of “message coherence” should not be underestimated.

Using the Clinician-Patient Relationship as Rehabilitation

There has been some important work on the complex patient-therapist interaction on the topic of chronic pain.30,31 The focus on pain as a threatening event or trust in the restorative capacity of the body is also a matter of observing how other people react to pain.32 Whether pain should provide a focus of assessment has been contentious since Fordyce's pioneering work.2 That attending to pain might increase pain itself is also a sensible argument, with some experimental support.33 However, the patient in pain is concerned most obviously about his or her pain and what it means, so to ignore it in treatment will only make sense if the patient understands the protective action model of pain. How, then, can we find the balance between honoring the patient's pain and suffering and integrating it with a rehabilitation process aimed at promoting function? We would contend that the unifying principle is to always be cognizant of providing evidence of safety. General aspects of interaction, such as being friendly and spending time concentrating on the patient's needs, provide evidence of safety. Demonstrating that you are informed, explaining pain, and explaining the rehabilitation approach in simple words also provide evidence of safety. Bearing in mind that much of our clinical practice involves reassurance34 and education, it would seem critical to ensure they are cornerstones of clinical training.35

Very relevant to this issue is the re-emergence of the patient narrative as a potential driver of rehabilitation and facilitator of conceptual and behavioral change. The idea that first-person accounts of pain have a role across the pain sciences—from the experimental laboratory36 to the clinic16 and to society at large37—is not necessarily new, but their re-emergence as bona-fide contributions to the field could very well open up new and novel methods to enhance the clinician-patient relationship as a therapeutic tool.

Should Pain Be Avoided in Rehabilitation?

That pain is not indicative of damage implies that intervention does not need to avoid pain altogether. Training should be time contingent or load contingent, not pain contingent. When pain increases during or after therapy, the patient can be reminded that this is not a symptom of damage but a protective strategy of an overly protective system. We have found that a balance is required between empathy and “holding the line” that adaptation, back to normal, will not occur without loading the system. Many principles of behavioral therapy are integral here as well—reminding patients that they are responsible for their body and for rehabilitation; encouraging patients to focus attention on healthy aspects of the body; integrating homework and occupational aspects for daily living; assisting patients in identifying sources of safety; learning and implementing coping skills and strategies to relieve pain—ultimately giving the patients the resources to master their own situation, to set attainable goals, and to learn the principles of slow and steady progression, all on the platform of a modern understanding of their pain.

Conclusion

We have proposed that current theoretical models of pain, and better understanding of increased sensitivity and decreased intracortical inhibition, compel us to approach pain rehabilitation, and its research, from the perspective of credible evidence of danger and safety. The clinician needs to be a keen observer and an empathic but assertive coach, ultimately providing patients with the skills to master their situation. The knowledge and evidence advocate a key role of Explaining Pain and highlight exciting developments in the application of modern pain science to the generation of new treatments.

Footnotes

  • Both authors provided concept/idea/project design and writing.

  • Dr Moseley receives author royalties for coauthored books that are relevant to some of the material in this article. Dr Lotze receives author royalities for editing a neurorehabilitation journal.

  • Dr Moseley's research was supported by a Principal Research Fellowship from the National Health and Medical Research Council, ID 1061279.

  • Received December 21, 2014.
  • Accepted April 2, 2015.
  • © 2015 American Physical Therapy Association

References

  1. ↵
    1. Vos T,
    2. Flaxman AD,
    3. Naghavi M,
    4. et al
    . Years lived with disability (YLDs) for 1160 sequelae of 289 diseases and injuries 1990-2010: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010 [erratum in: Lancet. 2013;381:628]. Lancet. 2012;380:2163–2196.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  2. ↵
    1. Main CJ,
    2. Keefe FJ,
    3. Jensen MP,
    4. et al
    . Fordyce's Behavioral Methods for Chronic Pain and Illness. Washington, DC: IASP Press; 2014.
  3. ↵
    1. Butler D,
    2. Moseley GL
    . Explain Pain. 2nd ed. Adelaide, Australia: Noigroup Publications; 2013.
  4. ↵
    1. Darwin F
    . The Life and Letters of Charles Darwin, Including an Autobiographical Chapter. Vol 1. London, United Kingdom: John Murray; 1887.
  5. ↵
    1. Moseley GL
    . Imagined movements cause pain and swelling in a patient with complex regional pain syndrome. Neurology. 2004;62:1644.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Moseley GL,
    2. Zalucki N,
    3. Birklein F,
    4. et al
    . Thinking about movement hurts: the effect of motor imagery on pain and swelling in people with chronic arm pain. Arthritis Rheum. 2008;59:623–631.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  7. ↵
    1. Moseley GL
    . Distorted body image in complex regional pain syndrome. Neurology. 2005;65:773.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Chalmers DJ
    . The Conscious Mind: In Search of a Fundamental Theory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1997.
  9. ↵
    1. Moseley GL,
    2. Arntz A
    . The context of a noxious stimulus affects the pain it evokes. Pain. 2007;133:64–71.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  10. ↵
    1. Necker L
    . Observations on some remarkable phenomena seen in Switzerland; and on an optical phenomenon which occurs on viewing a figure of a crystal or geometrical solid. Philos Mag Third Series. 1832;1:329–337.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  11. ↵
    1. Land EH
    . The retinex theory of color vision. Sci Am. 1977;237:108–128.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  12. ↵
    1. McGurk H,
    2. MacDonald J
    . Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature. 1976;264:746–748.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  13. ↵
    1. Oizumi M,
    2. Albantakis L,
    3. Tononi G
    . From the phenomenology to the mechanisms of consciousness: integrated information theory 3.0. PLoS Comput Biol. 2014;10:e1003588.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Wall P,
    2. McMahon S
    . The relationship of perceived pain to afferent nerve impulses. Trends Neurosci. 1986;9:254–255.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  15. ↵
    1. Kenshalo DR
    1. Melzack R,
    2. Casey KL
    . Sensory, motivational, and central control determinants of pain: a new conceptual model. In: Kenshalo DR, ed. The Skin Senses. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas Publisher; 1968:423–443.
  16. ↵
    1. Nicholas MK,
    2. Siddal P,
    3. Tonkin L,
    4. Beeston L
    . Manage Your Pain. Sydney, Australia: ABC Books; 2002.
  17. ↵
    1. Moseley GL
    . Combined physiotherapy and education is efficacious for chronic low back pain. Aust J Physiother. 2002;48:297–302.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  18. ↵
    1. Moseley GL
    . Joining forces; combining cognition-targeted motor control training with group or individual pain physiology education: a successful treatment for chronic low back pain. J Man Manip Ther. 2003;11:88–94.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  19. ↵
    1. Meeus M,
    2. Nijs J,
    3. Van Oosterwijck J,
    4. et al
    . Pain physiology education improves pain beliefs in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome compared with pacing and self-management education: a double-blind randomized controlled trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2010;91:1153–1159.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  20. ↵
    1. Van Oosterwijck J,
    2. Nijs J,
    3. Meeus M,
    4. et al
    . Pain neurophysiology education improves cognitions, pain thresholds, and movement performance in people with chronic whiplash: a pilot study. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2011;48:43–58.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Louw A,
    2. Diener I,
    3. Landers MR,
    4. Puentedura EJ
    . Preoperative pain neuroscience education for lumbar radiculopathy: a multicenter randomized controlled trial with 1-year follow-up. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2014;39:1449–1457.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  22. ↵
    1. Gallagher L,
    2. McAuley J,
    3. Moseley GL
    . A randomized-controlled trial of using a book of metaphors to reconceptualize pain and decrease catastrophizing in people with chronic pain. Clin J Pain. 2013;29:20–25.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  23. ↵
    1. Louw A,
    2. Diener I,
    3. Butler DS,
    4. Puentedura EJ
    . The effect of neuroscience education on pain, disability, anxiety, and stress in chronic musculoskeletal pain. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2011;92:2041–2056.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Clarke CL,
    2. Ryan CG,
    3. Martin DJ
    . Pain neurophysiology education for the management of individuals with chronic low back pain: systematic review and meta-analysis. Man Ther. 2011;16:544–549.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Lotze M,
    2. Moseley GL
    . Role of distorted body image in pain. Curr Rheumatol Rep. 2007;9:488–496.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Di Pietro F,
    2. McAuley JH,
    3. Parkitny L,
    4. et al
    . Primary motor cortex function in complex regional pain syndrome: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Pain. 2013;14:1270–1288.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Bowering KJ,
    2. O'Connell NE,
    3. Tabor A,
    4. et al
    . The effects of graded motor imagery and its components on chronic pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Pain. 2013;14:3–13.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  28. ↵
    1. Moseley GL
    . Graded motor imagery for pathologic pain: a randomized controlled trial. Neurology. 2006;67:2129–2134.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Johnson S,
    2. Hall J,
    3. Barnett S,
    4. et al
    . Using graded motor imagery for complex regional pain syndrome in clinical practice: failure to improve pain. Eur J Pain. 2012;16:550–561.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Miciak M,
    2. Gross DP,
    3. Joyce A
    . A review of the psychotherapeutic “common factors” model and its application in physical therapy: the need to consider general effects in physical therapy practice. Scand J Caring Sci. 2012;26:394–403.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Hadjistavropoulos T,
    2. Craig KD,
    3. Duck S,
    4. et al
    . A biopsychosocial formulation of pain communication. Psychol Bull. 2011;137:910–939.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Goubert L,
    2. Vlaeyen JW,
    3. Crombez G,
    4. Craig KD
    . Learning about pain from others: an observational learning account. J Pain. 2011;12:167–174.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  33. ↵
    1. Eccleston C,
    2. Crombez G
    . Pain demands attention: a cognitive-affective model of the interruptive function of pain. Psychol Bull. 1999;125:356–366.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  34. ↵
    1. Trager AC,
    2. Hubscher M,
    3. Henschke N,
    4. et al
    . Effect of primary care–based education on reassurance in patients with acute low back pain: systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Intern Med. In press.
  35. ↵
    1. Pincus T,
    2. Holt N,
    3. Vogel S,
    4. et al
    . Cognitive and affective reassurance and patient outcomes in primary care: a systematic review. Pain. 2013;154:2407–2416.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  36. ↵
    1. Thacker MA,
    2. Moseley GL
    . First-person neuroscience and the understanding of pain. Med J Aust. 2012;196:410–411.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Frank AW
    . The Wounded Storyteller. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 1995.
View Abstract
PreviousNext
Back to top
Vol 95 Issue 9 Table of Contents
Physical Therapy: 95 (9)

Issue highlights

  • Physical Therapists as Primary Practitioners in the Emergency Department
  • Kinesiophobia and Effects of Physical Therapy on Outcomes in Sciatica
  • Physical Therapists' Kinesiophobic Beliefs
  • Pregnancy-Related Low Back Pain
  • Gait Asymmetry Perception Poststroke
  • Functional Exercise Capacity in Survivors of Critical Illness
  • Novel Method for Inspiratory Muscle Training in Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease
  • Assessing Psychometric Properties in Chronic Pain and Fatigue
  • Movement System and General Exercise in Chronic Low Back Pain
  • Unilateral Lower Extremity Lymphedema
  • Hospital-Associated Deconditioning
  • Chronic Pain Rehabilitation
Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on JCORE Reference.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Theoretical Considerations for Chronic Pain Rehabilitation
(Your Name) has sent you a message from JCORE Reference
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the JCORE Reference web site.
Print
Theoretical Considerations for Chronic Pain Rehabilitation
Martin Lotze, G. Lorimer Moseley
Physical Therapy Sep 2015, 95 (9) 1316-1320; DOI: 10.2522/ptj.20140581

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Save to my folders

Share
Theoretical Considerations for Chronic Pain Rehabilitation
Martin Lotze, G. Lorimer Moseley
Physical Therapy Sep 2015, 95 (9) 1316-1320; DOI: 10.2522/ptj.20140581
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Technorati logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Connotea logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One
  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Old Lessons Ring True
    • Pain as a Conscious Motivator to Protect Our Body
    • Clinical Implications
    • The Dark Side of Plasticity
    • Retraining the Brain: Graded Motor Imagery
    • Using the Clinician-Patient Relationship as Rehabilitation
    • Should Pain Be Avoided in Rehabilitation?
    • Conclusion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Education Research in Physical Therapy: Visions of the Possible
  • Seven-Step Framework for Critical Analysis and Its Application in the Field of Physical Therapy
  • Medical Marijuana: Just the Beginning of a Long, Strange Trip?
Show more Perspectives

Subjects

  • Perspectives
  • Pain

Footer Menu 1

  • menu 1 item 1
  • menu 1 item 2
  • menu 1 item 3
  • menu 1 item 4

Footer Menu 2

  • menu 2 item 1
  • menu 2 item 2
  • menu 2 item 3
  • menu 2 item 4

Footer Menu 3

  • menu 3 item 1
  • menu 3 item 2
  • menu 3 item 3
  • menu 3 item 4

Footer Menu 4

  • menu 4 item 1
  • menu 4 item 2
  • menu 4 item 3
  • menu 4 item 4
footer second
footer first
Copyright © 2013 The HighWire JCore Reference Site | Print ISSN: 0123-4567 | Online ISSN: 1123-4567
advertisement bottom
Advertisement Top