Skip to main content
  • Other Publications
  • Subscribe
  • Contact Us
Advertisement
JCORE Reference
this is the JCORE Reference site slogan
  • Home
  • Most Read
  • About Us
    • About Us
    • Editorial Board
  • More
    • Advertising
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
    • Folders
    • Help
  • Patients
  • Reference Site Links
    • View Regions
  • Archive

Relationships Between Balance and Cognition in Patients With Subjective Cognitive Impairment, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Alzheimer Disease

Gro Gujord Tangen, Knut Engedal, Astrid Bergland, Tron Anders Moger, Anne Marit Mengshoel
DOI: 10.2522/ptj.20130298 Published 1 August 2014
Gro Gujord Tangen
G.G. Tangen, PT, MSc, Department of Health Sciences, Institute of Health and Society, University of Oslo, PO Box 1089 Blindern, Oslo 0317, Norway.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Knut Engedal
K. Engedal, MD, PhD, Norwegian Centre for Aging and Health, Vestfold Health Trust, Tønsberg, Norway.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Astrid Bergland
A. Bergland, PT, PhD, Oslo and Akershus University College of Applied Sciences, Oslo, Norway.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Tron Anders Moger
T.A. Moger, PhD, Department of Health Management and Health Economics, Institute of Health and Society, University of Oslo.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Anne Marit Mengshoel
A.M. Mengshoel, PT, PhD, Department of Health Sciences, Institute of Health and Society, University of Oslo.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Background Balance impairments are common in patients with Alzheimer disease (AD), but which aspects of balance are affected, at which stage of cognitive impairment, and their associations with cognitive domains remain unexplored.

Objectives The aims of this study were: (1) to explore differences in balance abilities among patients with subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) or mild cognitive impairment (MCI), mild AD, and moderate AD and (2) to examine the relationship between the various aspects of balance and cognitive domains.

Design This was a cross-sectional study.

Methods Home-dwelling patients with SCI or MCI (n=33), mild AD (n=99), and moderate AD (n=38) participated in this study. The Balance Evaluation Systems Test (BESTest), comprising 6 subscales—“Biomechanical Constraints,” “Stability Limits/Verticality,” “Anticipatory Postural Adjustments,” “Postural Responses,” “Sensory Orientation,” and “Stability in Gait”—was used to assess balance. Cognitive domains were assessed using the following measures: Mini-Mental Status Examination, Word-List Learning Test from the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD), Verbal Fluency Test, Clock Drawing Test, and Trail Making Test, parts A and B (TMT-A and TMT-B, respectively). Two-way between-group analyses of variance, adjusted for age, were used to analyze differences among the groups. Multiple linear regression analysis was used to explore the associations between balance and cognition.

Results Differences were found between the groups on all BESTest subscales; the moderate AD group had the worst scores. The TMT-B (measuring executive function) was associated with all of the BESTest subscales after controlling for demographic factors.

Limitations The cross-sectional design hampered interpretation of the development of balance impairments.

Conclusions The study findings indicate that all aspects of balance control deteriorate with increasing severity of cognitive impairment and that executive function plays an important role in balance control. Physical therapists should pay attention to these findings both in clinical practice and in future research.

Dementia is among the leading causes of disability and death in the elderly population. The majority (about 60%–70%) of people with dementia have Alzheimer disease (AD),1 and in the United States—with its increasing proportion of elderly people—the prevalence of AD is estimated to rise from 4.7 million in 2010 to 13.8 million in 2050.2 Alzheimer disease is a progressive neurodegenerative disease without any known cause or treatment. Symptoms of AD are associated with cognitive, psychiatric, and physical impairments and lead to inability to live independently, causing a heavy burden on both relatives and health care systems. Safe and independent mobility is essential to perform activities in everyday life and depends highly on intact balance control. Falls and fall-related injuries are more common in patients with AD than in their healthy peers.3–7 In general, reduced balance control and cognitive impairment are both well-established risk factors for falls among elderly people.8,9

The current dominant theory of balance control is the systems theory approach.10–13 Balance, in this approach, is seen as a result of complex integration and coordination of several underlying systems covering sensory/perceptual processes, cognitive influences (eg, attention, motivation, intention) and motor processes.11,13,14 Clinical studies have shown that impaired balance, as a general construct, is more common in patients with AD than in healthy controls15–19 and that impaired balance becomes more prevalent with increasing severity of AD.20 Experimental laboratory studies of patients with AD have examined single aspects of balance, and impaired sensory organization for balance control was found,21 as well as correlations between anterior-posterior postural sway and orientation for time and place16 and an increased impact of cognitive distraction on sway in patients with AD compared with healthy controls.22 However, for physical therapists, it is important to get a more nuanced understanding of how cognitive impairment may influence the various aspects of balance to inform their clinical work.

Physical therapists assess balance for various reasons: to determine the presence of a balance problem, to monitor changes, and to identify which aspects of balance control are affected. A recent study compared 25 patients with AD and 25 healthy elderly individuals on a broad set of balance measures that involved both quiet stance and dynamic tasks, as well as balance during a dual task.19 The authors concluded that balance impairments in patients with AD were of global character, as they performed worse than the controls on almost all measures.19 The recently developed Balance Evaluation Systems Test (BESTest) provides a framework for the assessment of aspects of balance control in line with the systems theory. The BESTest provides subscales developed to represent systems important for balance control as well as a total score.10 Accordingly, we used this assessment tool to obtain information on the various aspects of balance control of clinical relevance for physical therapists.

Given the lack of information about the various aspects of balance control in patients with AD, knowledge on deterioration of these aspects throughout the course of AD also is inadequate. People with subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) or mild cognitive impairment (MCI) have noticeable memory deficits but no functional impairment.23,24 Contrary to former beliefs,25,26 physical functions, such as gait speed, might start to decline simultaneously with, or even before, the decline in cognitive function.27,28 Furthermore, the degree of lower limb impairment (a global score of gait and balance tasks) in patients with MCI has been associated with the risk of later development of AD.29 Studies have shown worse balance performance in people with MCI than in healthy controls,16,30,31 although this finding has not been reported consistently in all studies.18

To our knowledge, no study has examined the differences in the underlying aspects of balance control between people with SCI or MCI and people with mild and moderate AD. This information may help to direct interventions to the affected aspects of balance and to decide at which stage of cognitive impairment interventions should be initiated. Cognitive function is similar to balance in that it comprises several interrelated domains such as memory, language, reasoning, visuospatial ability, and executive function. Executive function is not a unitary construct but includes attention, planning, set shifting, and flexible, goal-oriented behavior.32–34 Attention, defined as the individual's information-processing capacity, is thought to be particularly important for balance.35 Impaired executive function was associated with worse gait-related performances in people with cognitive impairment and dementia.36–39 In older adults without known cognitive impairments, poor executive function40 and poor visuospatial ability and memory41 were associated with an increased risk of falls. However, the relationship among the different aspects of balance, executive function, and other cognitive domains remains unexplored in people with AD. We suggest that such knowledge may generate new ideas on the content of balance interventions for people with cognitive impairment.

Therefore, this study addressed 2 aims: (1) to examine whether there were any differences among patients with SCI or MCI, mild AD, and moderate AD with regard to each of the subscales as well as the total score of the BESTest and (2) to examine which cognitive domains were associated with impaired balance control when controlled for demographic and health-related characteristics.

Method

Design and Participants

The present study had a cross-sectional design. Patients included in the Norwegian Dementia Registry (NDR) at Oslo University Hospital were screened consecutively for eligibility and, if deemed suitable, were invited to participate. Simultaneously, we also recruited patients through a local authority dementia team in a rural area outside Oslo. The inclusion period lasted from January 2011 to August 2012. To address the first aim of the study, we used the whole sample of included participants, whereas for the second aim, we used a subset of our participants who had completed all of the cognitive tests.

To be included in the study, the patients had to have a clinical diagnosis of SCI, MCI, or AD and had to be home dwelling. They needed to understand Norwegian and to be able to walk comfortably without a walking device. The diagnosis of SCI was used for patients who experienced subjective memory problems but still had normal performance on a comprehensive test battery at the memory clinic.42 The diagnosis of MCI was based on the Winblad criteria: subjective memory problems and a slight deterioration in the cognitive testing results but no functional impairment.24 The participants with SCI and MCI were analyzed as one group, as patients with these 2 diagnoses have memory complaints but still do not meet the criteria for dementia. We applied the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 10th edition, (ICD-10) diagnostic criteria for research to diagnose AD and to categorize patients with AD as having dementia of a mild or moderate degree.43 These criteria emphasize the ability of independent living to differentiate between mild and moderate stages of AD. Exclusion criteria were severe stage of AD, another diagnosis of dementia, severe hearing or vision impairments, neurologic conditions such as Parkinson disease, stroke with motor symptoms, or musculoskeletal conditions causing moderate to severe pain or impairing gait. An experienced geriatric psychiatrist (K.E.) who was blinded to the results of the balance testing reviewed the clinical diagnosis to ensure consistent use of the diagnostic criteria. All participants gave written informed consent prior to the study.

Demographic and Clinical Information

Information on demographic characteristics, medical conditions, medications, cognitive assessments, and walking habits were retrieved from the NDR or from the patients' medical records. We used the results from the cognitive testing done within the previous 6 months, and 80% of the balance assessments were done within 3 months of the cognitive testing. Additional tests were not performed if the results were not found in the NDR or the patient's medical record. The NDR does not contain information about musculoskeletal problems, so we additionally asked all patients: “Do you have any injury or condition, such as arthritis, hip prosthesis, or previous fractures, that may affect your balance?” We used this question as a dichotomous variable (yes/no) for the regression analysis. Information on physical activity was obtained through the walking habit questions: the number of days a week a patient walked outside multiplied by how long those walks generally lasted.44 The information from these questions and information from the NDR were based on agreement between the participants' own statements and those from their next of kin.

Cognitive Assessments

The Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE) was used to assess global cognition and consists of items concerning orientation, registration and recall, attention, language, following commands, and figure copying. The MMSE can be scored from 0 to 30, where a higher score indicates better performance.45 The Trail Making Test, part A (TMT-A) was used to test attention and processing speed.46 In this test, the participants were asked to draw a line between circles in increasing order (1–25). The Trail Making Test, part B (TMT-B) was used to test executive function and set shifting (the ability to go back and forth between multiple tasks).46 The participants were asked to draw a line to connect circles in increasing order and to alternate between numbers and letters (1–A–2–B–3…). Performances on both TMTs were timed, and attempts were stopped after 5 minutes. We divided the timed scores into 4 categories based on normative age-adjusted time intervals for both tests47: 0=cannot complete, 1=slower than −2 standard deviations of the norms, 2=between −1 and −2 standard deviations, and 3=better than −1 standard deviation. The ordinal scoring was used to avoid floor effects when participants were unable to complete the task.

The Clock Drawing Test (CDT) was used to evaluate visuoconstructive abilities.48 The participants were presented with a piece of paper with a circle and were asked to draw the numbers so that the circle looks like the face of a clock and then to draw the hands of the clock to read “10 after 11.” We used the 6-point scoring method as described by Shulman (0–5, where 5 is best).48

The Word-List Learning Test from the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD) was used to evaluate the learning aspect of memory.49 A list of 10 words was presented in 3 trials, and the scoring represents the total number of correct words learned (0–30, where 30 is best).

The Verbal Fluency Test from the CERAD was used to assess semantic verbal fluency.50 In this test, the participants were asked to name as many animals as possible within 1 minute.

The medical doctors at the memory clinic administered the cognitive tests. The cognitive tests have high interrater reliability in patients with dementia,45,48,50 but reliability has not yet been examined for patients with SCI. For the TMTs, reliability has not been established for people with dementia or SCI, but it is high for healthy people.51

Balance Assessment

Balance performance was evaluated with the BESTest, which was developed to target 6 underlying systems of balance control.10 The BESTest consists of 36 items grouped into 6 subscales: I—Biomechanical Constraints, II—Stability Limits/Verticality, III—Anticipatory Postural Adjustments, IV—Postural Responses, V—Sensory Orientation, and VI—Stability in Gait. Each item was scored on a 4-point ordinal scale, where 0 is the worst performance and 3 is the best performance. The sum scores for each subscale and the total score were converted to percentage scores (0%–100%, where 100% is best). The balance assessments were conducted in a quiet setting. All tasks were performed with the participants barefooted except for those in subscale VI (Stability in Gait). The physical therapist gave repeated instructions and demonstrations of the task when necessary. Verbal cueing was used if the participant hesitated in performing the timed tasks. In the last item, the Timed Up and Go Dual Task, we used the “random numbers” alternative as the cognitive task for all participants. We aimed to score the participants' first attempt. For the 3 items of compensatory stepping responses in subscale IV (Postural Responses), we allowed up to 3 attempts because many participants did not lean sufficiently beyond their stability limits at the first attempt. The same physical therapist (G.G.T.) tested all except 3 participants. High levels of test-retest and interrater reliability have been reported for the BESTest.10,52,53 So far, there have been no studies on the validity or reliability of the BESTest among populations with cognitive impairment.

Data Analysis

Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics version 20 (IBM Corp, Armonk, New York). The level of significance was set at P<.05. Descriptive statistics are reported as the mean and standard deviation when normally distributed or as the median with interquartile range if showing a skewed distribution. Numerator and percentages are reported for categorical variables. Differences between groups concerning patient characteristics were analyzed using chi-square tests for categorical variables and using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for normally distributed continuous variables. Comparisons between participants with complete and missing data for the regression analysis were performed using Student t, Mann-Whitney U, chi-square, or Fisher exact test.

To analyze the differences on each of the balance subscales adjusted for age among the groups of patients with SCI or MCI, mild AD, and moderate AD, we conducted a 2-way between-groups ANOVA. We controlled for age (categorized into 3 groups: 51–69, 70–79 and 80–92 years) because the group of participants with moderate AD was significantly older than the other 2 groups. We focused on the main effects of the 3 groups and performed Bonferroni post hoc comparisons to analyze any differences among the groups. As the first aim of our study was to examine differences among groups with different degrees of cognitive impairment, we did not perform further analysis of the interactions between groups and age. For the 3 subscales in the BESTest with skewed distributions (I, IV, and V), we also used Kruskal-Wallis tests (although it was not possible to adjust for age) to ensure that any differences among groups remained significant when using nonparametric tests.

To address the study's second aim, we used the subset of participants who had completed all of the cognitive tests. The associations between the BESTest scores and the cognitive tests, adjusted for demographic factors, were analyzed by multiple linear regression. Demographic factors were chosen based on clinical experience and results from previous studies. We checked bivariate correlations among the independent variables for colinearity, and we inspected the residual plots to ensure that model assumptions were not violated. The independent variables were entered into the models in 2 blocks. Block 1 contained the demographic factors that we aimed to control in our analyses: age, sex, comorbidity, education, and walking habits. Block 2 contained the cognitive measures: MMSE, 10 Word Test, CDT, Verbal Fluency Test, and TMT-A and TMT-B. To explore which cognitive measures were independently associated with the scores of the BESTest, we performed backward removal analyses on the variables in block 2: we removed the cognitive measures with lowest association one by one until only significant variables were left. The final model was evaluated through analysis of explained variance.

We then repeated the same strategy for multiple linear regression analyses for each of the BESTest subscales to examine which cognitive measures might be associated with the separate subscales.

Results

Of the 208 patients screened and eligible for our study, 181 (83.0%) consented to participate (Figure). Of those, 11 participants were later excluded because they did not fulfill the inclusion criteria or they were not able to follow the instructions throughout the entire BESTest. Thus, the sample for the ANOVA consisted of 170 participants: 33 in the SCI/MCI group (13 with SCI, 20 with MCI), 99 with mild AD, and 38 with moderate AD. Participant characteristics are presented in Table 1. The group of patients with moderate AD was older and had a lower education level than the other 2 groups.

Figure.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure.

Flow chart of participants' involvement in the study.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1.

Characteristics of Participants With Subjective or Mild Cognitive Impairment, Mild Alzheimer Disease, and Moderate Alzheimer Diseasea

Adjusted for age, the 3 groups performed significantly differently from each other on all 6 subscales of balance as well as on the total score (Tab. 2; eFigure). The largest differences among the groups were found in the “Stability in Gait” subscale (F=29.55, P<.001). Large effect sizes were seen for the total score (ηp2=0.24) and for the “Anticipatory Postural Adjustments” subscale (ηp2=0.18) and “Stability in Gait” subscale (ηp2=0.27), and the other subscales had medium effect sizes (ηp2= 0.07–0.10).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2.

Comparisons of Performance on the BESTest Among Participants With Subjective or Mild Cognitive Impairment, Mild Alzheimer Disease, and Moderate Alzheimer Diseasea

The subset with a complete dataset for the regression analysis consisted of 111 participants (65.3% of those included). Participants with moderate AD had the most missing data (Tab. 3). The main sources of missing data were the TMTs and the Word-List Learning Test, each with missing data for 30 or more participants.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 3.

Characteristics of Participants Included in the Regression Analysis and Comparison With Those Who Were Excluded From the Regression Analysis Because of Missing Dataa

All independent variables were associated with the BESTest total score in the bivariate analysis. The TMT-B was the only cognitive measure significantly associated with the BESTest total score after controlling for demographic factors (Tab. 4). Demographic factors explained 51% of the variance in the BESTest total score, and the TMT-B explained an additional 13% of the variance. The effect size (f2) of the final model was 1.94. For the corresponding multiple linear regression analysis with each of the BESTest subscales as the dependent variable, the TMT-B was consistently in the remaining subsets, in addition to the demographic factors of age and comorbidity (4 of the 6 subscales) (Tab. 5). The adjusted explained variance ranged from 0.20 for the “Sensory Orientation” subscale to 0.52 for the “Stability in Gait” subscale. The range of the effect sizes was from medium (f2=0.32) for the “Sensory Orientation” subscale to large for the other subscales (f2=0.54–1.22).

View this table:
Table 4.

Associations Between the Balance Evaluation Systems Test Total Score and Cognitive Assessments, Adjusted for Demographic Factors (n=111)a

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 5.

Associations Between the Subscales of the Balance Evaluation Systems Test and Cognitive Assessments, Adjusted for Demographic Factors (n=111)a

Discussion

The main findings of this study were that, for each subscale and for the total score of the BESTest, the mild AD group performed worse than the SCI/MCI group and that the moderate AD group performed worse than the other groups. Furthermore, we found that executive function, as measured by the TMT-B score, was associated with all of the BESTest subscales, even after adjusting for demographic factors.

The differences among the groups were not equally pronounced across all of the subscales. The highest scores in all 3 groups were found for the “Stability Limits,” “Sensory Orientation,” and “Postural Responses” subscales. The “Stability Limits” and “Sensory Orientation” subscales reflect aspects of balance during quiet stance, and our findings correspond well with those of previous studies. Increased postural sway has been observed across 3 stages of AD,54 and worse performance on sway velocity and limits of stability has been reported in patients with mild and moderate AD compared with healthy elderly people.19 In the 1-year follow-up, however, there were no differences in the rate of change of these measures between the healthy elderly group and patients with AD.55 Based on these previous results and the present findings, we suggest that although balance control while standing still is affected in people with AD, it is in general better preserved than dynamic balance control.

To our knowledge, postural responses involving reactive responses to external perturbation have not been examined separately in patients with AD. Being able to respond quickly and appropriately to slips and pushes is essential for avoiding a fall. A reduction in reactive responses is a typical trait of parkinsonism,56 and we were expecting severe impairments on this subscale because parkinsonian signs are commonly observed in patients with AD.57,58 The first attempts at tasks involving reactive responses are thought to give a greater disturbance to balance than subsequent attempts.59 As mentioned in the “Method” section, we allowed many of our participants several attempts at the compensatory stepping response items because they did not comprehend the instructions the first time. This approach might have resulted in these individuals receiving a better score than if they had had a valid first try, so we might have underestimated the impairments in this subscale.

The largest differences among the groups, and the lowest scores, were found in the “Stability in Gait” subscale. The items of this subscale examine stability and speed during walking in different challenging situations (such as when performing head turns and pivot turns and stepping over obstacles). Walking over obstacles has been examined previously, and patients with AD landed closer to the obstacle and were more likely to touch the obstacle during walking than were healthy controls.60 Our finding of apparent deterioration of gait performance in the present cross-sectional study when comparing the 3 groups is in line with findings from longitudinal studies: deterioration in gait-related measures was found both in home-dwelling patients with early-onset AD (aged less than 65 years)61 and in older patients with AD.62–64 Studies have even identified a slow gait speed as a predictor for cognitive decline among elderly people who are cognitively healthy.27,28 Our study extends the existing findings on gait function by indicating that balance in such tests also is altered, along with the severity of cognitive impairment.

To our knowledge, the present study is the first to use the BESTest among participants with dementia. We found the test feasible in that no negative incidents occurred during testing, and only 4/174 patients (2.3%) were unable to complete the test because they could not follow the instructions. The BESTest also was able to discriminate among all 3 groups. The BESTest total score is a global measure of balance control, and results can be associated with other clinical measures of balance such as the Berg Balance Scale (BBS).65 A previous study that used the BBS to assess balance showed no differences between individuals with mild AD and a healthy control group,66 and the authors commented that this finding could be explained by the well-known ceiling effect of the BBS.67 Our SCI/MCI group, with mean age of 68 years, performed slightly worse than healthy people between 60 and 69 years of age in a recent study.68 We are well aware of the limitations of a cross-sectional design, but our findings indicate a continuous deterioration of balance with increasing severity of cognitive impairment that should be further examined in longitudinal studies.

Our second question was to examine which cognitive domains were associated with impaired balance control when controlled for demographic factors. The demographic factors of age, sex, and comorbidity were all associated with balance, which is in line with previous findings.20 Global cognition, as measured by the MMSE, was highly correlated with the BESTest total score in the bivariate analysis but did not contribute to the final model. Executive function, as measured with the TMT-B, was still independently associated with both total score and each of the subscale scores of the BESTest. The only other cognitive test that remained in one of the final models was the Verbal Fluency Test, which was associated with the “Stability in Gait” subscale. This verbal task is often thought to reflect aspects of executive function as well, because those who perform well are likely to develop strategies for organizing their recall.34 Other studies have linked executive function, gait performance, and future fall risk in both healthy older people and people with dementia,36,69–71 but our study extends these findings to include balance control. However, increased postural sway was associated with reduced blood flow in the frontal lobes in a small cross-sectional study of patients with AD.54 Executive function also is closely linked to the functions of the prefrontal cortex in the frontal lobes, so that study might support our findings.

The TMT-B added 13% to the explained variance of the BESTest total score. This percentage may seem low, but it is above what we expected, and it is above the explained variance of the TMT-B on gait speed and of the Timed “Up & Go” Test in patients with MCI.37 The consistent association of executive function with each of the subscales as well as the total score underscores the control function of executive function. This finding also lends support to a nonrandomized study of older people with AD who demonstrated improvements on both balance performance and executive function after an intervention where they combined physical activity with cognitive tasks.72 We also suggest that special attention should be paid to those who exhibit impairments of executive function because they are likely to have balance impairments and be at risk for future falls.

A limitation of our study was the cross-sectional design, which relied heavily on a valid categorization of the participants into the 3 different groups. In addition, the physical therapist who tested the participants was not blinded to their preliminary diagnosis. However, we sought to reduce the risk of bias by assigning the diagnostic work to an experienced geriatric psychiatrist blinded to the results of the balance assessments. We ended up with unequal sizes of the groups, which could have caused us to make type II errors. However, we were still able to detect differences among the groups on every measure of the BESTest. We acknowledge that by treating SCI and MCI as one group, we had a very heterogeneous group where several patients will never develop dementia, as their memory complaints may have other reasons than incipient AD. Still, their memory complaints have caused them to seek medical attention, and we believe they contribute to how we interpret the relationship between balance and cognition in our study.

Another limitation in our study involved the missing data associated with the cognitive tests used in the regression analysis, which resulted in a smaller sample for the regression analysis compared with the ANOVAs. When individuals had severe problems in completing the MMSE and CDT, or displayed considerable lack of motivation during cognitive testing, most clinicians at the memory clinic chose to refrain from further testing out of ethical considerations. These concerns occurred most frequently in patients with severe cognitive impairments; therefore, the information was not missed at random. Consequently, we can only generalize the results from the regression analyses to patients with a mild to moderate degree of cognitive impairment.

The present study had several strengths, including the use of a comprehensive clinical test of balance in conjunction with evaluation of multiple cognitive domains. Furthermore, we found a consistent association between balance control and executive function, adjusted for demographic factors and in the presence of other relevant cognitive domains. Although most patients were recruited through the memory clinic, we believe that our results will be relevant for home-dwelling people with cognitive impairment and AD in the general population. The inclusion of patients with comorbidities such as arthritis or joint replacement would make it harder to associate the balance impairments with AD pathology. However, none of our patients used a walking device or experienced the testing as painful, and our findings were adjusted for the presence of comorbidities in the regression analysis. The clinical implications of the present study are that physical therapists should be aware that reduction in balance starts at an early stage of cognitive impairment, so it might be useful to initiate balance training before balance problems become too pronounced.

Future Directions

We found differences in all aspects of balance control reflected by the BESTest across patient groups with various levels of cognitive deficits. The worst balance performance was found in the group of patients with the most severe cognitive impairment. These findings are important, as the research within physical therapy focusing on these patient groups is yet at an early stage. Future longitudinal studies are needed to confirm the trajectory of deteriorating balance with the development of AD. The executive function component, as measured with the TMT-B, was associated with all aspects of balance control after controlling for demographic factors such as age, sex, and comorbidity.

The Bottom Line

What do we already know about this topic?

Balance impairments occur frequently in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. However, we have little information about which aspects of balance are affected and about their relationship to cognitive domains.

What new information does this study offer?

We found that performances on all 6 aspects of balance measured by the BESTest became increasingly worse with severity of cognitive impairment. Executive function was associated with all of these aspects of balance.

If you are a patient or a caregiver, what might these findings mean for you?

If a family member is starting to have cognitive impairments, the family may consider contacting a physical therapist for a balance assessment and advice about how to best preserve the individual's balance and mobility.

Footnotes

  • Ms Tangen, Dr Engedal, Dr Bergland, and Dr Mengshoel provided concept/idea/research design. Ms Tangen, Dr Moger, and Dr Mengshoel provided data analysis. Ms Tangen, Dr Engedal and Dr Mengshoel provided project management. Dr Engedal provided institutional liaisons. Ms Tangen and Dr Mengshoel provided fund procurement. Ms Tangen and Dr Engedal provided study participants, data collection, and facilities/equipment. All authors provided writing and consultation (including review of manuscript before submission).

  • The authors thank all of the participants for their cooperation in this study and the staff at Oslo University Hospital, Charlotta Hamre and Åse S. Slemdal, for participation in the data collection.

  • The Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics in the Southeast of Norway approved the study.

  • A poster presentation of the results of this study, in part, was given at the Joint World Congress of the International Society for Posture & Gait Research, Gait and Mental Function; June 22–26, 2013; Akita, Japan.

  • Received July 17, 2013.
  • Accepted April 14, 2014.
  • © 2014 American Physical Therapy Association

References

  1. ↵
    1. Fratiglioni L,
    2. Launer LJ,
    3. Andersen K,
    4. et al
    ; Neurologic Diseases in the Elderly Research Group. Incidence of dementia and major subtypes in Europe: a collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurology. 2000;54:S10–S15.
    OpenUrl
  2. ↵
    1. Hebert LE,
    2. Weuve J,
    3. Scherr PA,
    4. Evans DA
    . Alzheimer disease in the United States (2010–2050) estimated using the 2010 census. Neurology. 2013;80:1778–1783.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  3. ↵
    1. Allan LM,
    2. Ballard CG,
    3. Rowan EN,
    4. Kenny RA
    . Incidence and prediction of falls in dementia: a prospective study in older people. PLoS One. 2009;4:e5521.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Buchner DM,
    2. Larson EB
    . Falls and fractures in patients with Alzheimer-type dementia. JAMA. 1987;257:1492–1495.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  5. ↵
    1. Morris JC,
    2. Rubin EH,
    3. Morris EJ,
    4. Mandel SA
    . Senile dementia of the Alzheimer's type: an important risk factor for serious falls. J Gerontol. 1987;42:412–417.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Shaw FE
    . Falls in cognitive impairment and dementia. Clin Geriatr Med. 2002;18:159–173.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  7. ↵
    1. Tolppanen AM,
    2. Lavikainen P,
    3. Soininen H,
    4. Hartikainen S
    . Incident hip fractures among community dwelling persons with Alzheimer's disease in a Finnish nationwide register-based cohort. PLoS One. 2013;8:e59124.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Muir SW,
    2. Gopaul K,
    3. Montero Odasso MM
    . The role of cognitive impairment in fall risk among older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Age Ageing. 2012;41:299–308.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Tinetti ME,
    2. Speechley M,
    3. Ginter SF
    . Risk factors for falls among elderly persons living in the community. N Engl J Med. 1988;319:1701–1707.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  10. ↵
    1. Horak FB,
    2. Wrisley DM,
    3. Frank J
    . The Balance Evaluation Systems Test (BESTest) to differentiate balance deficits. Phys Ther. 2009;89:484–498.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Woollacott MH,
    2. Shumway-Cook A
    . Changes in posture control across the life span: a systems approach. Phys Ther. 1990;70:799–807.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Huxham FE,
    2. Goldie PA,
    3. Patla AE
    . Theoretical considerations in balance assessment. Aust J Physiother. 2001;47:89–100.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  13. ↵
    1. Shumway-Cook A,
    2. Woollacott M
    . Motor Control: Translating Research Into Clinical Practice. 3rd ed. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2007.
  14. ↵
    1. Mancini M,
    2. Horak FB
    . The relevance of clinical balance assessment tools to differentiate balance deficits. Eur J Phys Rehabil Med. 2010;46:239–248.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Franssen EH,
    2. Souren LE,
    3. Torossian CL,
    4. Reisberg B
    . Equilibrium and limb coordination in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1999;47:463–469.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  16. ↵
    1. Leandri M,
    2. Cammisuli S,
    3. Cammarata S,
    4. et al
    . Balance features in Alzheimer's disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. 2009;16:113–120.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  17. ↵
    1. Pettersson AF,
    2. Engardt M,
    3. Wahlund LO
    . Activity level and balance in subjects with mild Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2002;13:213–216.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  18. ↵
    1. Pettersson AF,
    2. Olsson E,
    3. Wahlund LO
    . Motor function in subjects with mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2005;19:299–304.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  19. ↵
    1. Suttanon P,
    2. Hill KD,
    3. Said CM,
    4. et al
    . Balance and mobility dysfunction and falls risk in older people with mild to moderate Alzheimer disease. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. 2012;91:12–23.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Mazoteras Muñoz V,
    2. van Abellan KG,
    3. Cantet C,
    4. et al
    . Gait and balance impairments in Alzheimer disease patients. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2010;24:79–84.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Chong RK,
    2. Horak FB,
    3. Frank J,
    4. Kaye J
    . Sensory organization for balance: specific deficits in Alzheimer's but not in Parkinson's disease. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1999;54:M122–M128.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Manckoundia P,
    2. Pfitzenmeyer P,
    3. d'Athis P,
    4. et al
    . Impact of cognitive task on the posture of elderly subjects with Alzheimer's disease compared to healthy elderly subjects. Mov Disord. 2006;21:236–241.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Reisberg B,
    2. Prichep L,
    3. Mosconi L,
    4. et al
    . The pre-mild cognitive impairment, subjective cognitive impairment stage of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2008;4:S98–S108.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  24. ↵
    1. Winblad B,
    2. Palmer K,
    3. Kivipelto M,
    4. et al
    . Mild cognitive impairment—beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med. 2004;256:240–246.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  25. ↵
    1. Ala TA,
    2. Frey WH
    . Validation of the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria regarding gait in the clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease: a clinicopathologic study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1995;9:152–159.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  26. ↵
    1. McKhann G,
    2. Drachman D,
    3. Folstein M,
    4. et al
    . Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology. 1984;34:939–944.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Mielke MM,
    2. Roberts RO,
    3. Savica R,
    4. et al
    . Assessing the temporal relationship between cognition and gait: slow gait predicts cognitive decline in the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2012;68:929–937.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  28. ↵
    1. Buracchio T,
    2. Dodge HH,
    3. Howieson D,
    4. et al
    . The trajectory of gait speed preceding mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:980–986.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  29. ↵
    1. Aggarwal NT,
    2. Wilson RS,
    3. Beck TL,
    4. et al
    . Motor dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment and the risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:1763–1769.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  30. ↵
    1. Boyle PA,
    2. Wilson RS,
    3. Buchman AS,
    4. et al
    . Lower extremity motor function and disability in mild cognitive impairment. Exp Aging Res. 2007;33:355–371.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  31. ↵
    1. Delbaere K,
    2. Kochan NA,
    3. Close JC,
    4. et al
    . Mild cognitive impairment as a predictor of falls in community-dwelling older people. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;20:845–853.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  32. ↵
    1. Elliott R
    . Executive functions and their disorders. Br Med Bull. 2003;65:49–59.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    1. Jurado MB,
    2. Rosselli M
    . The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol Rev. 2007;17:213–233.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  34. ↵
    1. Lezak M,
    2. Howieson D,
    3. Bigler E,
    4. Tranel D
    . Neuropsychological Assessment. 5th ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2012.
  35. ↵
    1. Woollacott M,
    2. Shumway-Cook A
    . Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture. 2002;16:1–14.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  36. ↵
    1. Allali G,
    2. Assal F,
    3. Kressig RW,
    4. et al
    . Impact of impaired executive function on gait stability. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;26:364–369.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. McGough EL,
    2. Kelly VE,
    3. Logsdon RG,
    4. et al
    . Associations between physical performance and executive function in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: gait speed and the Timed “Up & Go” Test. Phys Ther. 2011;91:1198–1207.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Sheridan PL,
    2. Solomont J,
    3. Kowall N,
    4. Hausdorff JM
    . Influence of executive function on locomotor function: divided attention increases gait variability in Alzheimer's disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51:1633–1637.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  39. ↵
    1. Kearney FC,
    2. Harwood RH,
    3. Gladman JR,
    4. et al
    . The relationship between executive function and falls and gait abnormalities in older adults: a systematic review. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2013;36:20–35.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Buracchio TJ,
    2. Mattek NC,
    3. Dodge HH,
    4. et al
    . Executive function predicts risk of falls in older adults without balance impairment. BMC Geriatr. 2011;11:74.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Martin K,
    2. Thomson R,
    3. Blizzard L,
    4. et al
    . Visuospatial ability and memory are associated with falls risk in older people: a population-based study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2009;27:451–457.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  42. ↵
    1. Braekhus A,
    2. Ulstein I,
    3. Wyller TB,
    4. Engedal K
    . The Memory Clinic: outpatient assessment when dementia is suspected. Tidsskr Nor Laegeforen. 2011;131:2254–2257.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. Diagnostic criteria for research. 1993. Available at: http://www.who.int/classifications/icd/en/GRNBOOK.pdf. Accessed March 13, 2013.
  44. ↵
    1. Hörder H,
    2. Skoog I,
    3. Frändin K
    . Health-related quality of life in relation to walking habits and fitness: a population-based study of 75-year-olds. Qual Life Res. 2013;22:1213–1223.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  45. ↵
    1. Folstein MF,
    2. Folstein SE,
    3. McHugh PR
    . “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  46. ↵
    1. Reitan RM
    . The relation of the Trail Making Test to organic brain damage. J Consult Psychol. 1955;19:393–394.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Ivnik RJ,
    2. Malec JF,
    3. Smith GE,
    4. et al
    . Neuropsychological tests' norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE token, WRAT-R reading, AMNART, STROOP, TMT, and JLO. Clin Neuropsychol. 1996;10:262–278.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  48. ↵
    1. Shulman KI
    . Clock-drawing: is it the ideal cognitive screening test? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;15:548–561.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  49. ↵
    1. Fillenbaum GG,
    2. van Belle G,
    3. Morris JC,
    4. et al
    . Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD): the first twenty years. Alzheimers Dement. 2008;4:96–109.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  50. ↵
    1. Morris JC,
    2. Heyman A,
    3. Mohs RC,
    4. et al
    . The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD), part I: clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1989;39:1159–1165.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  51. ↵
    1. Fals-Stewart W
    . An interrater reliability study of the Trail Making Test (parts A and B). Percept Mot Skills. 1992;74:39–42.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  52. ↵
    1. Padgett PK,
    2. Jacobs JV,
    3. Kasser SL
    . Is the BESTest at its best? A suggested brief version based on interrater reliability, validity, internal consistency, and theoretical construct. Phys Ther. 2012;92:1197–1207.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    1. Leddy AL,
    2. Crowner BE,
    3. Earhart GM
    . Functional gait assessment and balance evaluation system test: reliability, validity, sensitivity, and specificity for identifying individuals with Parkinson disease who fall. Phys Ther. 2011;91:102–113.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  54. ↵
    1. Nakamura T,
    2. Meguro K,
    3. Yamazaki H,
    4. et al
    . Postural and gait disturbance correlated with decreased frontal cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1997;11:132–139.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  55. ↵
    1. Suttanon P,
    2. Hill KD,
    3. Said CM,
    4. Dodd KJ
    . A longitudinal study of change in falls risk and balance and mobility in healthy older people and people with Alzheimer disease. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. 2013;92:676–685.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Horak FB,
    2. Dimitrova D,
    3. Nutt JG
    . Direction-specific postural instability in subjects with Parkinson's disease. Exp Neurol. 2005;193:504–521.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  57. ↵
    1. Scarmeas N,
    2. Hadjigeorgiou GM,
    3. Papadimitriou A,
    4. et al
    . Motor signs during the course of Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2004;63:975–982.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  58. ↵
    1. Wilson RS,
    2. Bennett DA,
    3. Gilley DW,
    4. et al
    . Progression of parkinsonian signs in Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 2000;54:1284–1289.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  59. ↵
    1. Allum JH,
    2. Tang KS,
    3. Carpenter MG,
    4. et al
    . Review of first trial responses in balance control: influence of vestibular loss and Parkinson's disease. Hum Mov Sci. 2011;30:279–295.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Alexander NB,
    2. Mollo JM,
    3. Giordani B,
    4. et al
    . Maintenance of balance, gait patterns, and obstacle clearance in Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1995;45:908–914.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  61. ↵
    1. Tangen GG,
    2. Londos E,
    3. Olsson J,
    4. et al
    . A longitudinal study of physical function in patients with early-onset dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2012;2:622–631.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Cedervall Y,
    2. Kilander L,
    3. Aberg AC
    . Declining physical capacity but maintained aerobic activity in early Alzheimer's disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2012;27:180–187.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    1. Wittwer JE,
    2. Webster KE,
    3. Menz HB
    . A longitudinal study of measures of walking in people with Alzheimer's Disease. Gait Posture. 2010;32:113–117.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  64. ↵
    1. Hebert LE,
    2. Bienias JL,
    3. McCann JJ,
    4. et al
    . Upper and lower extremity motor performance and functional impairment in Alzheimer's disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2010;25:425–431.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  65. ↵
    1. Berg KO,
    2. Wood-Dauphinée SL,
    3. Williams JI,
    4. Maki B
    . Measuring balance in the elderly: validation of an instrument. Can J Public Health. 1992;83(suppl 2):S7–S11.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  66. ↵
    1. Kato-Narita EM,
    2. Nitrini R,
    3. Radanovic M
    . Assessment of balance in mild and moderate stages of Alzheimer's disease: implications on falls and functional capacity. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2011;69:202–207.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Blum L,
    2. Korner-Bitensky N
    . Usefulness of the Berg Balance Scale in stroke rehabilitation: a systematic review. Phys Ther. 2008;88:559–566.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  68. ↵
    1. O'Hoski S,
    2. Winship B,
    3. Herridge L,
    4. et al
    . Increasing the clinical utility of the BESTest, Mini-BESTest, and BriefBESTest: normative values in Canadian adults who are healthy and aged 50 years and over. Phys Ther. 2014;94:334–342.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  69. ↵
    1. Mirelman A,
    2. Herman T,
    3. Brozgol M,
    4. et al
    . Executive function and falls in older adults: new findings from a five-year prospective study link fall risk to cognition. PLoS One. 2012;7:e40297.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Ble A,
    2. Volpato S,
    3. Zuliani G,
    4. et al
    . Executive function correlates with walking speed in older persons: the InCHIANTI study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:410–415.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  71. ↵
    1. Hausdorff JM,
    2. Schweiger A,
    3. Herman T,
    4. et al
    . Dual-task decrements in gait: contributing factors among healthy older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008;63:1335–1343.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  72. ↵
    1. Pedroso RV,
    2. Coelho FG,
    3. Santos-Galduroz RF,
    4. et al
    . Balance, executive functions and falls in elderly with Alzheimer's disease (AD): a longitudinal study. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2012;54:348–351.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
View Abstract
PreviousNext
Back to top
Vol 94 Issue 8 Table of Contents
Physical Therapy: 94 (8)

Issue highlights

  • Gait-Related Outcomes in Youth With Neuromuscular Diagnoses
  • Trigger Point Dry Needling for Plantar Heel Pain
  • A Multidisciplinary Treatment Guideline for de Quervain Disease
  • Pain Sensitivity in Individuals With Spine Pain
  • Balance and Cognition in Patients With Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer Disease
  • Experiences of Exercise During Pregnancy
  • Activity and Blood Pressure in Children With Cerebral Palsy
  • Hip Abductor Strength and Physical Function After Unilateral Total Knee Arthroplasty
  • Sedentary Behavior and Youth With Cerebral Palsy
  • Maximal Exercise Testing in Multiple Sclerosis
  • Treadmill Training After Surgical Removal of Spinal Tumor in Infancy
  • Postmilitary Adjustment to Civilian Life
Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on JCORE Reference.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Relationships Between Balance and Cognition in Patients With Subjective Cognitive Impairment, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Alzheimer Disease
(Your Name) has sent you a message from JCORE Reference
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the JCORE Reference web site.
Print
Relationships Between Balance and Cognition in Patients With Subjective Cognitive Impairment, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Alzheimer Disease
Gro Gujord Tangen, Knut Engedal, Astrid Bergland, Tron Anders Moger, Anne Marit Mengshoel
Physical Therapy Aug 2014, 94 (8) 1123-1134; DOI: 10.2522/ptj.20130298

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Download Powerpoint
Save to my folders

Share
Relationships Between Balance and Cognition in Patients With Subjective Cognitive Impairment, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Alzheimer Disease
Gro Gujord Tangen, Knut Engedal, Astrid Bergland, Tron Anders Moger, Anne Marit Mengshoel
Physical Therapy Aug 2014, 94 (8) 1123-1134; DOI: 10.2522/ptj.20130298
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Technorati logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Connotea logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One
  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Method
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Reliability and Validity of Force Platform Measures of Balance Impairment in Individuals With Parkinson Disease
  • Predictors of Reduced Frequency of Physical Activity 3 Months After Injury: Findings From the Prospective Outcomes of Injury Study
  • Effects of Locomotor Exercise Intensity on Gait Performance in Individuals With Incomplete Spinal Cord Injury
Show more Research Reports

Subjects

Footer Menu 1

  • menu 1 item 1
  • menu 1 item 2
  • menu 1 item 3
  • menu 1 item 4

Footer Menu 2

  • menu 2 item 1
  • menu 2 item 2
  • menu 2 item 3
  • menu 2 item 4

Footer Menu 3

  • menu 3 item 1
  • menu 3 item 2
  • menu 3 item 3
  • menu 3 item 4

Footer Menu 4

  • menu 4 item 1
  • menu 4 item 2
  • menu 4 item 3
  • menu 4 item 4
footer second
footer first
Copyright © 2013 The HighWire JCore Reference Site | Print ISSN: 0123-4567 | Online ISSN: 1123-4567
advertisement bottom
Advertisement Top