Abstract
The plasticity of the brain is an increasingly important topic for physical therapists interested in childhood development, learning, and repair following injury. The study of plasticity directly in the human nervous system presents numerous challenges, such as the ability to assess neuronal function in vivo because of physical impediments, such as the skull, skin, and dura. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), however, has become a suitable, noninvasive, and painless technique that can be applied to detect changes in cortical excitability as an indicator of neurological changes. Furthermore, repetitive trains of TMS themselves can induce plasticity. This article discusses the use of TMS to investigate and manipulate plasticity in the human nervous system.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was introduced by Barker et al in 19851 and has since gained recognition as a safe, relatively painless, and noninvasive method for mapping cortical motor representation in both normal and pathologic cases.2–7 Recently, TMS was used to investigate the possible mechanisms underlying both spontaneous and therapy-induced motor recovery after stroke. Over the last decade, numerous studies explored the therapeutic potential of repetitive TMS (rTMS) for the treatment of a variety of psychiatric diseases.8 Despite the large number of studies that have investigated the efficacy of rTMS, significant questions remain about the way in which it may be used in physical therapist clinical practice. Because of the proliferation of these studies and uncertainties that remain, a review of TMS and plasticity is timely.
This article is intended to serve several purposes that are presented in a logical sequence. First, we explore the fundamental nature and mechanisms of plasticity. This discussion is followed by a brief introduction to TMS techniques and physiological effects of magnetic stimulation in healthy adults. We then apply this foundation to TMS studies of plasticity in subjects who were healthy. Next, we describe how TMS induces plasticity within the human brain. This description is followed by a survey of stimulation techniques that can serve as potential therapeutic tools for promoting favorable plasticity, initially within a variety of neurological disorders and subsequently within the process of neurological rehabilitation. Finally, we look toward the future, speculating on how novel and far-reaching approaches with TMS could influence human brain plasticity. As a result, readers should have a firm understanding of neuroplasticity that may lead to a better understanding of the human nervous system and the relevance of neuroplasticity to clinical rehabilitation.
Fundamentals of Plasticity
Although there is generally no universally accepted definition of “plasticity,” the term may be thought of either as the capacity of the brain to change or as an intrinsic property of the human nervous system that persists throughout the life span.9 Here we address only a subcategory of the pervasive phenomenon of plasticity; the mutability of skeletal muscle is described in the article by Segal in this Special Series. In most experimental situations, plasticity is defined neurophysiologically by changes in stimulus-response characteristics following direct cortical stimulation. Classen and Ziemann, in discussing stimulation-induced plasticity in the human motor cortex, observed that “neuronal plasticity may be defined as any functional change within the nervous system outlasting an (experimental) manipulation.”10(p135) In this context, they noted that there is no general agreement on how long an effect needs to outlast the intervention but reasonably assert that “plasticity is usually only applied when neuronal changes outlast the manipulation by more than a few seconds.”10(p135)
If plasticity does characterize the capacity of the brain to change and is an intrinsic but persistent property of the nervous system, one relevant application of the underlying principles may involve the acquisition of new skills, especially in response to changes in the environment. This mechanism may be the basis for growth, development, and learning. For example, when a new skill is acquired, the function of the neural network is determined by the most dominant input that it receives; the input can be altered by certain behaviors. Additionally, and particularly germane to the practice of neurorehabilitation scientists across disciplines, there is the possibility that plastic changes underlie the mechanisms by which the recovery of function occurs after central nervous system (CNS) or peripheral nervous system injury11,12; this point is explored more completely later.
It is known that the functional organization of the cerebral cortex is plastic; that is, changes in organization occur throughout the life span in response to the numerous events that define experiences. The potential for reorganization has been demonstrated in both sensory and motor areas of the adult cortex as a consequence of trauma, pathological changes, manipulation of sensory experience, or learning. These changes can be evaluated only when referenced against an extensive collection of experimental data that have identified a repeatable representation pattern (eg, somatotopic, tonotopic, or retinotopic pattern) from which changes can be detected. Although assessing the scope of such changes is often at the edge of current technical capabilities, there are striking examples of significant and rapid changes, such as the increased size of the trained hand motor representation following 5 consecutive days of piano exercise (2 hours per day) compared with the size of the untrained hand motor representation.13
Alterations in cortical organization are known to emerge through changes in synaptic efficacy within the cortex and elsewhere in the nervous system. Furthermore, these changes have been linked closely to 2 phenomena, long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD). Long-term potentiation, the long-lasting enhancement of synaptic transmission first reported by Bliss and colleagues more than 30 years ago,14,15 has been the focus of an enormous amount of investigation (Fig. 1). Figure 1 shows that there is a clear, long-lasting potentiation (up to 4 hours) of responses following trains of stimuli given at 15 per second to the hippocampal formation of awake, active rabbits. Long-term potentiation has long been regarded, along with its counterpart, LTD, the weakening of a neuronal synapse that lasts from hours to days, as a potential mechanism for memory formation and learning. Figure 2 shows a model of normal synaptic transmission (Fig. 2A) and the rise of Ca2+ levels in the dendritic spine, triggering the induction of LTP (Fig. 2B).
Discovery of long-term potentiation, which was first reported for the perforant path (entorhinal cortex-dentate gyrus in the hippocampal formation). e.p.s.p.=excitatory postsynaptic potential. (Reprinted with permission from Bliss TV, Gardner-Medwin AR. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the unanaestetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol. 1973;232:357–374. ©1973 The Physiological Society.)
Model for the induction of long-term potentiation. According to this model, N-methyl-D-aspartic (NMDA) and non–NMDA (quisqualate/kainate) receptor channels (Q/K) are located near each other in dendritic spines. (Reprinted with permission of The McGraw-Hill Companies from Kandel ER. Cellular mechanisms of learning and the biological basis of individuality. In: Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM, eds. Principles of Neural Science. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Elsevier; 1991:1009–1031. Kandel adapted the figure from Gustafsson B, Wigstrom H. Physiological mechanisms underlying long-term potentiation. Trends Neurosci. 1988;11:156–162.)
Possible Mechanisms for Plasticity
Evidence for candidate mechanisms to support cortical plasticity at the population and cellular levels has been proposed and evaluated. Mechanisms proposed to support rapid plasticity include uncovering of latent or existing connections, activation of existing but silent synapses, activity-dependent synaptic plasticity, and generalized excitability changes in postsynaptic neurons. Morphological changes, such as neurogenesis, synaptogenesis, and synaptic remodeling, require time for full expression and, therefore, may be involved preferentially in providing new cortical areas for further changes. Evidence exists for the operation of most of these mechanisms during development, during learning, or in response to injury. Moreover, these mechanisms are not mutually exclusive; different mechanisms might operate simultaneously or in some serial order.
Uncovering of latent or existing connections.
Uncovering or unmasking preexisting connections in the primary motor cortex (M1)16,17 could be a mechanism for rapid (early) plasticity in response to manipulations of sensory inputs18,19 or motor outputs20,21 of cortical representational maps.
The somatosensory cortex of adult mammals has been shown to have the capacity to reorganize itself when inputs are removed through cutting of afferent nerves or amputation of a part of the body. The area of the cortex that normally would respond to stimulation from the missing input can become responsive to inputs from other parts of the body surface. Plastic changes were shown to occur in the primary somatosensory cortex of the flying fox following amputation of the single exposed digit on the forelimb.22 Immediately after amputation, neurons in the area of the cortex receiving input from the missing digit were not silent but responded to stimulation from adjoining regions of the digit, hand, arm, and wing. In the week following amputation, the enlarged receptive fields shrank until they covered only the skin around the amputation wound. The immediate response was interpreted as a removal of inhibition, and the subsequent shrinking of the fields might have been attributable to reestablishment of the inhibitory balance in the affected cortex and its inputs.
Activation of existing but silent synapses.
Alternatively, the activation of existing but silent synapses could serve as a mechanism for the induction of rapid plasticity. Silent synapses are connections between neurons displaying no α-amino-5-hydroxy-3-methyl-4-isoxazole propionic acid (AMPA)–mediated glutamate response23,24; presynaptic transmitter release would not result in a rapid potential shift in the target neuron. The AMPA receptor is a non-N-methyl-D-aspartic–type ionotropic transmembrane receptor for glutamate that mediates fast synaptic transmission in the CNS. Its name is derived from its ability to be activated by the artificial glutamate analog AMPA. Receptors for AMPA are found in many parts of the brain and are the most commonly found receptors in the nervous system. The “awakening” of silent synapses by the insertion of postsynaptic AMPA receptors25–28 is a mechanism proposed to account for the rapid increases in synaptic efficacy that have been observed experimentally.
Silent synapses have been implicated in brain plasticity in both young and mature animals.29 There is convincing evidence for the occurrence of silent synapses in the developing nervous system,23,24 but as maturation progresses, silent synapses become rare27,30 and presumably are replaced by active ones. The unmasking of any silent synapses that are present could support functional reorganization. The silent synapse mechanism may be relevant to the immature human nervous system and hence rehabilitation potential in young patients with cerebral palsy but is a less likely candidate mechanism for the cortical changes seen in older adults during recovery from stroke.
Activity-dependent synaptic plasticity and LTP.
The most widely studied but controversial mechanism for supporting representational plasticity is LTP,14,31 especially as a critical link between behavioral change and synaptic function. For the hippocampal cortex, neocortex, and amygdala, there is now more than 30 years of evidence supporting a possible role of LTP in learning and memory. Population measures of neuronal cells have indicated that LTP and LTD operate during learning to modify synaptic efficacy.32 Certain forms of learning lead to an enhancement of synaptic responses in a variety of brain structures.33–35 Recently, LTP was implicated in the learning of new motor skills,36 and there is compelling evidence that LTP is the mechanism involved in natural learning. In the study by Rioult-Pedotti et al,36 rats were trained for 5 successive days to reach with their preferred forelimb into a box and retrieve small food pellets. Grasp attempts began during the first session, and the success rate improved during the first 3 training days and then became asymptotic (Fig. 3A). The results reported by Rioult-Pedotti et al36 indicated that increased synaptic efficacy with initial skill learning as well as skill performance was maintained. Learning specifically strengthened extracellular field potentials in the M1 forelimb region (Fig. 3B). There were no interhemispheric differences in the hind limb region or in paired control rats. The data suggested that synapses are modifiable; they are modified with learning and are strengthened through an LTP-like mechanism.
Learning a new motor skill potentiates synaptic responses in primary motor cortex (M1) horizontal connections. (A) Learning was defined as the success rate over time, which was the ratio of the number of consumed pellets (Pcons) to the number of retrieved pellets (Ptot) ± the SEM. Numbers above data points represent the numbers of animals trained for various numbers of days. A randomly chosen subset of animals trained for 5 days was used for subsequent electrophysiological recordings. (B) Learning specifically strengthened extracellular field potentials (FP) in the M1 forelimb (FL) region. There were no interhemispheric differences in the hind-limb region (HL) or in paired control rats (PC). Means ± SEMs were from 4 times the threshold intensity and were normalized to the untrained (untr) or right (ri) hemisphere. ampl.=amplitude, le=left, tr=trained. (Modified with permission of AAAS from Rioult-Pedotti MS, Friedman D, Donoghue JP. Learning-induced LTP in neocortex. Science. 2000;290:533–536. Copyright 2000, AAAS.)
New Synapse Formation
In addition to occurring through LTP, increases in synaptic efficacy also could occur if learning induced the formation of new synapses, as has been reported after the occurrence of lesions in the visual and somatosensory cortexes37,38 and after learning in the motor cortex.39 The formation of new synapses or the remodeling of existing synapses has long been thought to be a fundamental mechanism of learning and memory at the cellular level.40–42 For example, motor skill learning has been shown to increase the number of synapses per neuron in the motor cortex39 and the cerebellum.43–45
Enriched Environments
Exposure to enriched environments results in a larger number of synapses per neuron,46 increases in dendritic spine density,47 and changes in dendritic spine morphology.48,49 In a study by Biernaskie and Corbett,50 animals that had lesions and that were exposed to an enriched environment showed enhanced dendritic spine complexity and length compared with animals exposed to a “standard” environment. These results suggested that enrichment combined with task-specific rehabilitative therapy is capable of augmenting intrinsic neuronal plasticity within noninjured, functionally connected brain regions as well as promoting an enhanced functional outcome.
Thus far, the general concept of cortical plasticity as it pertains to the motor cortex and its role in motor skill learning and more general principles concerning synaptic plasticity have been introduced. The possible mechanisms for plasticity have been discussed as they relate to motor system function, skill learning, and rehabilitation. These constructs underlying plasticity are precursors to the description of a noninvasive technique, TMS, a tool that provides a valuable method for exploring and understanding cortical plasticity in humans.
Examples of Plastic Change
Some of the most convincing evidence that learning and practice influence cortical organization and that learning operates through LTP- and LTD-mediated mechanisms has been obtained in studies of the motor cortex. This work is significant to physical therapists because knowing that potentiation has been engaged implies that the impaired or damaged motor cortex can be restructured through appropriate physical rehabilitation methods or through other means (eg, pharmacological or magnetic stimulation) that alter the mechanisms accounting for LTP and LTD.
The functional topography (ie, the graphic delineation of spatial architecture of the cortex usually on 2-dimensional maps) of M1 can be modified by peripheral or central injury, electrical stimulation, pharmacological manipulations, or experience. Behaviorally or experimentally induced reorganization of M1 output maps is characterized by shifts in borders between different motor representations. For example, M1 representations undergo rapid reorganization within hours of the occurrence of peripheral nerve lesions.20,51,52 Changes in cortical output maps can be induced with prolonged changes in limb positions,53,54 supporting the conclusion that sensory feedback derived from joint or muscle afferents is important in shaping M1 representations.
The primary motor cortex is also a site in which reorganization occurs during the acquisition or practice of motor skills. In a study in which intracortical microstimulation techniques (and not TMS) were used, skilled finger use in monkeys expanded the digit representation in M1.55 Skill learning–induced changes in M1 also were detected at the single-cell level in primates.56 Monkeys learned to adapt their reaching movements to externally applied force fields. The firing rate and the tuning of individually recorded cells in M1 changed during the period of adaptation to new force fields. A group of these cells (memory cells) retained the newly acquired activation pattern even after the force field was shut off and the monkey's hand trajectory returned to the control condition. Other memory cells that normally were directionally untuned became directionally tuned with the acquisition of the new skill and remained tuned to the direction of the arm movement after the force field was shut off. These data provide evidence for single-cell plasticity in M1. In humans, M1 representations also appeared to be enlarged or rearranged during motor learning.57–60 Further, roles of M1 in early motor consolidation61 and in motor memory58 have been demonstrated in humans.
In rats, learning a skilled reaching task but not an unskilled reaching task led to significant increases in the mean areas of the wrist and digit representations at the expense of the size of the shoulder representation; these results demonstrated that training-induced map reorganization was characterized by an expansion of “trained” representations into “untrained” representations without an overall increase in map size62 as rats accrued skilled distal forelimb movements. Moreover, such changes may well be driven by the specificity or “challenge” contained within the task, such as the skill set required to reach. For example, Kleim et al63 demonstrated a significant increase in the volume of neurons within the caudal forebrain area of rats trained to retrieve food pellets from a rotating disk (skilled reaching), but this result was not obtained when rats used a total forelimb lever press to obtain the food reward (unskilled reaching).
These results indicated that representational plasticity is driven by skill acquisition, learning, or practice of a newly acquired action and not by simple repetitive motor activity64,65 and suggested that only specific patterns of activity are capable of producing functional M1 plasticity. The implications of these observations for the provision and progression of rehabilitative training procedures are profoundly clear.
Introduction to TMS Techniques and Physiological Effects on Adults Who Are Healthy
Much of the work on basic mechanisms of plasticity in humans has been done with TMS techniques. These procedures have been performed on the motor cortex, in which the response to each stimulus is relatively easy to quantify through the use of the amplitude of a motor evoked potential (MEP) response. Transcranial magnetic stimulation is based on Faraday's principle of mutual induction: Electrical energy can be converted into magnetic fields and magnet fields can be converted into electric energy.66 About 60 years later, d'Arsonval described the production of phosphenes with exposure to a magnetic field.67 In 1985, Barker et al1 introduced modern TMS, which has since gained recognition as a safe,68 relatively painless, and noninvasive method for mapping cortical motor representations in both normal and pathologic situations.2–7 Recently, TMS was used to investigate possible mechanisms underlying both spontaneous and therapy-induced motor recovery after stroke.69,70
Electromagnetic induction allows current to be directed through a handheld copper stimulation coil, which produces a transient magnetic field (Fig. 4B). When held over the scalp, the rapidly changing magnetic field induces a small electric current (Fig. 4E) in underlying brain tissue; this current produces a depolarization of nerve cells that results in the stimulation or disruption of brain activity, depending on the frequency and intensity of stimulation as well as the location of the stimulating probe. When applied over M1 at low stimulus intensities, single-pulse TMS is thought to stimulate the corticospinal tract indirectly (transsynaptically) through horizontal fiber depolarization.71,72 The neurons activated depend on the size, shape, orientation, and intensity of the stimulus waveform that are produced by the magnetic stimulator.73 The resultant efferent volleys can be recorded as MEPs with surface or indwelling electrodes at peripheral target muscles.
Principles of transcranial magnetic stimulation. The current in the coil generates a changing magnetic field (B) that induces an electric field (E) in the brain. The drawing at the upper right depicts motor cortex stimulation and the trajectory of the pyramidal axons. At the microscopic level, the electric field affects the transmembrane potentials and may lead to local membrane depolarization and subsequent neural activation. Macroscopic responses can be detected with functional imaging tools (electroencephalography [EEG], functional magnetic resonance imaging [fMRI], positron emission tomography [PET], and single-photon emission computed tomography [SPECT]), with surface electromyography (EMG), or as behavioral changes. (Adapted from Ruohonen J, Ilmoniemi RJ. Physical principles for transcranial magnetic stimulation. In: Pascual-Leone A, Davey NJ, Rothwell J, et al, eds. Handbook of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation. New York, NY: Arnold; 2002:17–29. Reproduced by permission of Edward Arnold (Publishers) Ltd.)
Transcranial magnetic stimulation may be applied as a single stimulus or may be repeated many times per second. In most studies, either round or figure-eight coils are used. Figure-eight coils consist of 2 round coils placed side by side, producing more focal stimulation. Coils with a small diameter have a more focused field of stimulation but require a higher stimulus intensity to produce a similar depth of field of penetration. Highly focused stimulation is essential for many research applications, although uncertainty exists about whether this property will prove clinically useful, because less focused stimulation may compensate better for variations in the locations of pathological lesions and interindividual anatomy.
Biphasic stimulus pulses are more efficient in stimulating the brain than monophasic pulses, even when the initial phase of the stimulus is the same size,74,75 because the charge transfer is maximal in the swing between the first and second phases of a biphasic pulse.73
The delivery of TMS often is described on the basis of the frequency of the cortical stimulation. Repetitive (or rapid-rate) TMS usually refers to the application of TMS at frequencies above 1 Hz and often is applied in treatment studies (see below). The application of TMS at frequencies of 1 Hz or below may be referred to as slow or low-frequency TMS and often is used in motor cortex mapping procedures.8
In the context of physical therapy, the need to understand the relationship between an intervention and its effect on movement capabilities would make TMS a most appealing tool for studying cortical reorganization. Different TMS parameters are used to investigate motor system excitability. The “hot spot” (the most active scalp position for the target muscle) for motor stimulation is defined as the location at which the minimal stimulus intensity needed to produce an evoked motor response (the motor threshold) is the lowest from among all of the locations surveyed but at which the highest-amplitude response at that stimulus intensity also is obtained.76 Specifically, the resting motor threshold for the hot spot is defined as the minimum TMS intensity required to elicit at least 5 MEPs (≥50 μV) in 10 consecutive stimuli at rest.77
A principal measure is the area of motor output representation, often referred to as an MEP map. The MEP map refers to the area on the scalp surface from which MEPs in the target muscle can be obtained. For this, multiple scalp sites are stimulated by moving the stimulation coil along a grid. Other important measures include MEP latency, location of the amplitude-weighted center of gravity (COG) of the motor output map,4 MEP amplitudes (at rest and sometimes with facilitation), and MEP recruitment curves.65,78,79 The location of the COG of the MEP map corresponds to the scalp location at which the largest number of the most excitable corticospinal neurons can be stimulated. Therefore, changes in the COG should indicate true changes in the topographical organization of motor cortex representations.
Therapeutic studies in which TMS is used as an outcome measure have been undertaken, and an examination of their relative strengths and weaknesses seems appropriate because the data generated from such studies have profound implications for the interpretation of cortical reorganization following the application of neurorehabilitative procedures.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation has become appreciated as an important treatment modality for a variety of psychiatric diseases, including major depressive disorder, schizophrenia, and obsessive-compulsive disorder.8 Transcranial magnetic stimulation also has become an important evaluative tool80 and potential predictor of stroke recovery.81 However, TMS currently is not approved by the US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) for the treatment of these disorders in the United States. Single-pulse TMS has achieved FDA approval for the stimulation of peripheral nerves and muscles in the United States (but not for use on the brain). Therefore, TMS can be used as a tool for the evaluation of nerve root and plexus lesions. In November 2005, licensing for magnetic stimulation was granted by Health Canada for the assessment of neurological and muscular functions. Here we describe the use of TMS-derived mapping as an outcome measure.
As mentioned previously, the MEP map refers to the area on the scalp surface from which MEPs in the target muscle can be obtained. For this, multiple scalp sites are stimulated by moving the stimulation coil along a grid. Motor evoked potentials are recorded from electrodes placed strategically over a muscle of interest as maps related to specific movements are charted (Fig. 5). Transcranial magnetic stimulation mapping of motor cortical areas follows the basic principles of Penfield82 and is based on the idea of stimulating different regions of the brain and measuring the motor effects. Maps are generated by quantifying the motor effects and relating these to the scalp sites stimulated. Such maps indicate the region of the scalp in which stimulation can evoke a response in a muscle of interest and, therefore, are related only indirectly to the origins of the projection in the underlying motor cortex.83 Motor evoked potentials are elicited by providing a temporally varying current passed through a coil to induce an electric field in the underlying brain when the coil is placed over the appropriate cortical location, such as the motor cortex. When the MEPs are displayed as a function of a Cartesian coordinate system, a motor map can be created (Fig. 6).
Responses evoked in the extensor digitorum communis muscle by transcranial magnetic stimulation of the motor cortex at rest. Stimulation is delivered at 100 milliseconds. The motor evoked potential begins at approximately 120 milliseconds.
Three-dimensional map of the representation of the extensor digitorum communis (EDC) muscle after stimulation of a patient who, 6 months previously, had sustained an infarct in the left posterior limb of the internal capsule that resulted in right upper-limb paresis. The map was made either before (top row) or after (bottom row) 2 weeks of constraint-induced movement therapy. The x-y grid represents the surface of the contralateral scalp, divided into 1-cm squares. The height of the bar on each square indicates the mean peak-to-peak electromyographic response (microvolts) evoked at each point on the scalp for 10 stimuli delivered at 0.2 Hz. Before intervention, electromyographic responses on the side of the stroke (stimulation of the left hemisphere, right upper panel) were smaller than those on the intact side (left upper panel). However, after constraint-induced therapy, motor evoked potentials were larger in the impaired muscle (right lower panel) and were obtained from a wider area of the scalp than those of the less impaired left hand (left lower panel). Stimulation was set to be 10% above the resting motor threshold. A response required that at least 5 of 10 stimuli exceeded a peak-to-peak amplitude of 50 μV.
However, the interpretation of MEP maps generated by TMS has its limitations. The expectation that one can localize individual muscles by stimulating the motor cortex is erroneous. Transcranial magnetic stimulation mapping lacks the obvious precision found in microelectrode recordings. Using single-cell studies, investigators have shown that the organization of the primate motor cortex is much less discrete than that of the primary sensory areas. Individual corticospinal neurons project to several muscles, and the projections to any one muscle may be spread through a wide area of the cortex and intermix with projections to neighboring muscles.84 The result is a mosaic in which general patterns of hand, arm, and shoulder projections can be distinguished but in which true boundaries are imprecise.
This anatomical certainty is important because the electrode placement traditionally used during TMS studies may lead to cortical MEPs that may be derived from more than one muscle85; therefore, the relationship of the stimulation to the response should not necessarily be expressed as a muscle but as a movement. Moreover, if electrodes used to record MEPs are not placed specifically over the muscle in question, then the volume of the conducted response actually may represent accumulated responses from several muscles.86 This concern is justified because monitoring of the cortical representation of movement before and after an intervention with TMS as the assessment vehicle should yield data relevant to the intent of the treatment. Thus, for example, a treatment designed to relax finger, thumb, or wrist flexion motions while enhancing the counterpart extension activities should be reflected in MEPs that include the relevant movements and not the counterproductive movements.87,88
As mentioned above, TMS-derived maps are related only indirectly to the origins of the projection in the underlying motor cortex. Nevertheless, TMS-derived maps can provide at least a gross idea of the somatotopic pattern of the human motor cortex and reveal the best points for activating muscles in the shoulder, arm, and hand as well as in the face, arm, and leg.4 The process of finding the best points for activating various muscles can be standardized by marking a matrix of points on the scalp and then plotting the amplitudes of electromyographic responses obtained in various muscles at each point with a Cartesian coordinate system (Fig. 6).
Such maps provide 3 pieces of information: the optimal position at which to obtain the largest response (the so-called hot spot), the COG of the area, and the area of the scalp from which responses can be obtained. The optimal position, or hot spot, presumably corresponds to the location of the most excitable population of neurons that project to the target muscle. Specific details regarding the derivation of these measures can be found elsewhere.4,86
The area of the representation is more complex and depends on 2 factors: the true area of the cortex on which neurons that project to the target muscle are located and the stimulus intensity used to produce the map. If the stimulus intensity is too low, then the total extent of the map may be underestimated because less excitable elements will not be recruited. If the stimulus intensity is too high, then the area will be overestimated because the stimulus current will spread beyond the point of stimulation.89 These 2 opposing factors are difficult to reconcile with TMS, leaving the exact meaning of the map area that is recorded difficult to interpret.73
Nearly all mapping studies recognize these inherent limitations in technique; therefore, they focus not on the absolute size of a map but on changes in the map resulting, for example, from a stroke or intervention. With the exception of a few studies, most interventions change the size of the map without affecting its hot spot.90–94 In such cases, changes in the map size are best observed when participants are in a relaxed position and the muscles being tested are in a state of inaction. However, observations of motor excitability at rest present challenges to interpretation. In a recent study, Darling et al95 showed that the variability of motor potentials evoked by TMS depended on muscle activation. They showed that the relative variability of single MEPs at a constant stimulus intensity and prestimulus muscle electromyographic activation was lower during maintained 5% and 10% contraction levels than during 0% contraction levels.95 Therefore, maintaining a stable low-intensity contraction helps to stabilize cortical and spinal excitability. This observation suggests that comparisons of physiological changes during recovery in people with neuropathology may be influenced by the resting state of muscle contraction.
TMS for Predicting Functional Recovery After Stroke
Transcranial magnetic stimulation has been used to predict functional recovery after stroke, with general agreement that, in the acute phase after stroke, the inability to elicit MEPs following focal stimulation of the affected hemisphere correlates with poor functional outcome.96 The persistence of contralateral MEPs in the acute phase, regardless of the clinical grade of the patient's deficit, is a good marker of a favorable outcome.97–99 Furthermore, the latencies of responses elicited in the acute phase are prolonged, and these latencies shorten in duration in a manner that is highly correlated with muscle strength and hand function test scores as a patient recovers function.100,101
Several investigators have examined the correlation between TMS-derived map characteristics after stroke and the extent of motor recovery in humans.102–104 Pennisi et al81 demonstrated that complete hand paralysis in association with the absence of early MEPs (within 48 hours of ictus) predicted poor neurological recovery at 1 year in 15 subjects after stroke (middle cerebral artery infarct). Conversely, the preservation of motor potentials evoked by TMS in the early period after stroke may portend good functional recovery.72,105 Other investigators72,81,106,107 have reported relationships between the rate and extent of recovery after stroke and changes in the presence of MEPs, length of time for conduction from cortex to muscle, MEP latency, excitability threshold, and MEP amplitude. The absence of a response to TMS, a long duration of MEP latency, and a lengthened conduction time (compared with those of people who are healthy) in the early period after an injury are predictive of reduced hand motor function recovery.
In monohemispheric infarctions, decreased affected hemisphere motor output area and increased excitability thresholds for paretic muscles have been observed repeatedly in TMS-derived maps obtained during the subacute and chronic phases for patients with stroke.70,108 These electrophysiological changes presumably are related to motor impairment and may be secondary to neuronal damage, disuse, unbalanced transcallosal inhibition from the less affected hemisphere, or other, unidentified mechanisms.109
Responses to Repetitive Task Practice
Results from recent work with animal models suggested that the specificity and difficulty of training may affect the extent of use-dependent cortical plasticity.63,110–112 Similar findings were reported for motor recovery in patients after stroke. Liepert et al108 examined the effect of one intensive session of physical therapy in 9 subjects at 4 to 8 weeks after stroke. Participants received 1.5 hours of manual dexterity exercises in addition to ongoing “standard” therapy. Transcranial magnetic stimulation mapping of the abductor pollicis brevis (APB) muscle representation was performed 1 week before, immediately before, immediately after, and 1 day after the training session.
The area of APB muscle representation in the affected hemisphere increased significantly immediately after training but then decreased toward baseline after 1 day. Increased affected hemisphere motor output area was associated with improved dexterity on a clinical measure (the Nine-Hole Peg Test) in 7 of the subjects, although the amount of clinical improvement did not correlate with the extent of change in the area. The excitability threshold at the hot spot and the COG were unchanged after training, possibly signifying that the enlargement in the affected hemisphere was attributable to increased excitability at the edges of the map. The rapid change detected in the TMS-derived map after a brief training session suggested that functional, rather than structural, mechanisms were involved. Potential mechanisms discussed by the authors included the modulation of inhibitory gamma-aminobutyric acid transmission at the borders of the motor map and alterations in glutamate transmission.108 Gamma-aminobutyric acid is the chief inhibitory neurotransmitter in the vertebrate CNS.
TMS-Derived Mapping in Constraint-Induced Movement Therapy (CIMT)
In recent studies, TMS-derived mapping was used to investigate the effects of CIMT on the more affected upper extremity. Liepert et al113 used focal TMS to construct cortical output maps of the APB muscle in 6 patients with chronic stroke before and after 10 days of CIMT. Constraint-induced movement therapy involves the immobilization of the unaffected upper limb in tandem with intense training of the affected limb. The basis of this intervention is that it overcomes “learned nonuse,” which has been described as a limiting factor in patients with hemiparesis.114–116 As noted in earlier studies of subjects after stroke, significantly higher motor thresholds, smaller MEP amplitudes, and smaller areas of excitable cortex were observed in the affected hemisphere.
After CIMT, TMS parameters showed no change in thresholds but significant increases in MEP amplitudes and APB muscle motor output area in the affected hemisphere, possibly indicating increased excitability of surrounding neuronal networks. The unaffected hemisphere motor output areas were smaller after the training period, presumably because of decreased use of the less affected upper extremity, normalization of the unaffected hemisphere APB muscle representation, or increased transcallosal inhibition of the unaffected hemisphere by the affected hemisphere.
Shifts in the COG were significant (in the medial-lateral axis) only for the affected hemisphere, suggesting the possible recruitment of adjacent areas along the motor cortex. All subjects showed significant improvement in their use of the affected extremity, but scores on the Motor Activity Log,117 a 6-point subjective impression of how well and how often movement is observed in the affected arm during basic activities of daily living, did not correlate with the degree of map changes. Liepert et al108 suggested that physical therapy induces use-dependent reorganization which supports recovery-associated plastic changes.
In a follow-up study,70 clinical (Motor Activity Log) and TMS measurements were obtained at multiple time points before and after CIMT in 13 patients with chronic stroke (>6 months). Again, the affected hemisphere showed a smaller area of APB muscle representation at baseline, with a near doubling of the area after CIMT. Motor Activity Log improvements were maintained at the later measurement points. However, a return toward baseline in the area of the affected hemisphere APB muscle representation was seen at the 4-week and 6-month TMS sessions, indicating a possible “normalization after therapy-induced hyper-excitability”70 through improved synaptic efficacy or the relegation of motor function to non–TMS-accessible regions.
Several mechanisms have been purported to explain the TMS-derived map changes observed following CIMT.70 The intervention may have produced long-lasting changes in the cortical inhibitory network or perhaps the enhancement of synaptic strength within preexisting synaptic connections. Given that the unaffected limb was immobilized during the intervention, it is possible that the initial changes were related to a use-dependent mechanism.
Induction of Plasticity With Cortical Stimulation
The plasticity of the CNS has attracted much interest from the rehabilitation community because of its presumed relationship to mechanisms underlying the learning of new skills. The exact neural basis for motor skill acquisition has not been established completely but appears to be dependent on a neural network that includes several cortical and subcortical structures.118–122 Although coordinated activity across cortical regions may be necessary for successful motor learning, there is evidence for the critical role of the motor cortex in the early consolidation of learned skills.57,61 As noted previously, a balancing of LTP and LTD between cortical regions has been proposed as the most probable mechanism mediating motor learning.123 The excitability of the motor system may be modified by external stimulation involving the repetitive application of TMS pulses directed to M1 or other areas of the brain.
rTMS
Repetitive TMS is a series of magnetic pulses that temporarily summate and change neural activities to a greater degree than traditional single-pulse TMS.124 Repetitive TMS can modulate the excitability of the motor cortex beyond the period of stimulation.125,126 This modulation is dependent on various factors but, in general, high-frequency (≥3 Hz) rTMS has been shown to increase contralateral motor cortex excitability, whereas low-frequency (≤1 Hz) rTMS decreases contralateral motor cortex activity (MEP). Mechanisms similar to LTP and LTD are thought to be involved in the generation of these effects.127–129 High-frequency rTMS increases overall corticospinal synaptic activity,130 as expressed through changes in blood flow and metabolism and as measured by positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging, whereas low-frequency rTMS tends to reduce synaptic activity in targeted brain areas.126 Moreover, it seems that modulatory effects extend beyond a targeted area and involve various cortical and subcortical regions functionally related to the targeted area.126,131
Modulation of the Motor Cortex
Down-Regulation of the Intact Motor Cortex
Plasticity within the affected motor cortex may be enhanced or activity within the intact motor cortex may be down-regulated. One way in which to enhance motor function in a paretic hand may be through the down-regulation of activity in the ipsilateral, intact motor cortex (with the purpose of reducing abnormal inhibition from the intact hemisphere to the affected hemisphere) (Fig. 7, item 5). In addition to local effects under the stimulated location, stimulation applied to a given site can induce distant effects on cortical function and behavior.130 For example, rTMS applied to M1 in one hemisphere elicits changes in activation in positron emission tomography scans in M1 in the opposite hemisphere. Low-frequency rTMS applied to the motor cortex in one hemisphere down-regulates motor cortex excitability in the homonymous motor representation in the opposite hemisphere,132 consistent with the concept of a physiological balance of reciprocal inhibitory projections between the hemispheres.
Possible strategies for influencing hand function. (1) A decrease in somatosensory input from the intact hand, as in cutaneous anesthesia or the use of a mitt in constraint-induced movement therapy (CIT), leads to performance improvements in the nonanesthesized hand in healthy volunteers (indicated by downward circled arrow in lower left panel). (2) An increase in somatosensory input from the paretic hand, for example, through the use of somatosensory stimulation or CIT, may improve motor function (indicated by upward circled arrow in lower right panel). (3) Produce anesthesia of a body part proximal to the paretic hand (downward circled arrow, labeled 3). (4) Plasticity within the affected motor cortex may be enhanced (upward circled arrow, labeled 4, in upper left panel). (5) Activity within the intact motor cortex may be down-regulated (downward circled arrow, labeled 5, in upper right panel). rTMS=repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation, tDCS=transcranial direct current stimulation. (Modified with permission of the American Medical Association from Ward NS, Cohen LG. Mechanisms underlying recovery of motor function after stroke. Arch Neurol. 2004;61:1844–1848. Copyright 2004, American Medical Association. All rights reserved.)
Recent studies showed that when patients attempted voluntary movement of a paretic hand, the interhemispheric inhibitory connections were disturbed. Specifically, some of these patients showed an abnormally high interhemispheric inhibitory drive from M1 in the intact hemisphere to M1 in the affected hemisphere,133 a finding that was more prominent in more impaired patients. The idea of applying low-frequency rTMS to the unaffected hemisphere to improve motor function in patients was tested recently.134 In a group of 10 (5 test and 5 control) patients, those receiving rTMS (1 Hz, 100% of the motor threshold, 600 pulses to the unaffected hemisphere over M1) showed a significant decrease in simple and choice reaction times and improved performance in the Purdue Pegboard Test with their affected hand after 3 sessions. A similar stimulation protocol applied to the premotor cortex produced no significant results.
Up-Regulation of the Affected Motor Cortex
Enhancement of the ability of peri-infarcted and non–primary motor cortex regions of the affected hemisphere to respond to motor training or other neurorehabilitative interventions may be important because recent observations showed that increases in a number of growth-related processes likely contribute to behavioral recovery (Fig. 7, item 4). These processes may take place at the rim of tissue surrounding a cortical infarct.110,135–137 Cortical stimulation can modify activity in the motor cortex in animals138 and modulates cortical plasticity in humans. For example, TMS synchronously applied to a human motor cortex engaged in a motor training task enhanced use-dependent plasticity in the contralateral hand.139 This outcome provides evidence for the role of TMS in enhancing use-dependent plasticity and has implications for treatment methods aimed at facilitating motor recovery after stroke. This notion was tested recently in a therapeutic trial. Repetitive TMS or sham stimulation was applied over the stroke-affected motor cortex daily for 10 days to 2 randomly assigned groups of 26 patients with acute ischemic stroke. Patients otherwise continued their normal treatments. Disability measures—such as the Scandinavian Stroke Scale, the National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale, and the Barthel Index—applied before rTMS, at the end of the last rTMS session, and 10 days later showed that rTMS stimulation improved patients’ scores more than sham stimulation.140
The implication of these findings is that noninvasive cortical stimulation could represent an adjuvant to motor training in efforts to recover lost function after cortical lesions such as stroke. Consistent with this view, recent studies showed that noninvasive transcranial direct current stimulation can enhance motor function in people who are able-bodied141 and patients with chronic stroke.142
Given that there are several options for increasing and decreasing the levels of excitability and synaptic activation of the motor cortex in order to promote and facilitate plastic changes and consequently to improve motor learning in people who are healthy and in people with stroke, effects similar to those obtained with direct electrical cortical stimulation (through surgically implanted epidural electrodes) without the risks inherent in surgery might be expected. Moreover, rTMS might effectively replace the need for surgical procedures in at least a subset of patients. Although transcranial direct current stimulation has been shown to be effective, it produces a current that is dispersed through the cortex, thereby posing a challenge to the identification of muscle-specific changes and the exact anatomical substrate influenced by the stimulus.
TMS as a Potential Therapeutic Tool for Promoting Beneficial Plasticity
Repetitive TMS might be considered a therapeutic tool because it produces effects on the cerebral cortex that outlast the stimulus. It is assumed that, in some cases, it may be possible to manipulate these lasting effects either to reverse the pathological processes responsible for the condition or to change the excitability of remaining healthy systems so that they can compensate for the underlying disturbance.
It is clear that TMS can produce effects not only at the site of stimulation but also at distant connected sites. Thus, stimulation of M1 affects spinal motor neurons and muscle through at least 2 synaptic linkages. The same is true of central connections. For example, stimulation over the motor cortex in one hemisphere affects the excitability of contralateral motor areas through transcallosal connections,130,143 stimulation over the frontal eye fields affects metabolic activity in the parieto-occipital cortex,131 and stimulation over the premotor cortex affects the excitability of M1.144
The effect of TMS is proportional to the level of neuronal excitability at the time at which the stimulus is applied. Thus, motor potentials evoked in actively contracting muscles are larger than those evoked in muscles at rest. The same principle applies to central pathways. The excitability of the transcallosal connections between the motor cortexes changes depending on whether people contract one hand or both hands while performing a task. This mechanism suggests the possibility of increasing the specificity of targeting of particular connections by applying rTMS when a person performs a behavioral task. For example, if a person with upper-limb hemiparesis had poor individual finger movements but finger tapping was maintained, then the application of stimulation during finger tapping might prove beneficial to the individual finger movements because of the mechanism outlined. For a further discussion of the mechanism involved, refer to work done by Liepert et al,145 Shimizu et al,146 and De Gennaro et al.147
Perhaps the most problematic question regarding the therapeutic use of rTMS concerns the duration of its effect. In all studies of participants who were healthy, effects have lasted between 30 minutes and 1 hour. The limited period of time following the removal of rTMS during which to modulate the excitability of the motor cortex has led several groups148–151 to use repeated (daily) administration of rTMS to prolong benefits through the summation of responses.
Five consecutive sessions of rTMS increased the magnitude and duration of the motor effects in patients with stroke.152 Fifteen patients with chronic stroke were randomly assigned to receive active or sham rTMS of the unaffected hemisphere. Compared with sham rTMS, active rTMS resulted in a significant improvement in motor function performance in the affected hand that lasted for 2 weeks. There was a significant correlation between improvement in motor function performance and change in corticospinal excitability in the affected hemisphere. These results support and extend the findings of previous studies of rTMS in patients with stroke because 5 consecutive sessions of rTMS increased the magnitude and duration of the motor effects.
Heightened excitability typically is expressed through electrophysiological differences, but few studies have addressed behavioral enhancements of the contralateral limb. Thus, work on the motor system commonly has used MEP threshold, MEP amplitude, paired-pulse testing, or silent-period duration as a measure of the effects of rTMS. However, relatively few studies have tested whether any of these measures is behaviorally relevant. In subjects who were healthy, finger tapping speed decreased after low-frequency magnetic stimulation at 0.9 Hz for 15 minutes (810 pulses) over the motor cortex.153 In contrast, peak force and peak acceleration were not affected by application to the hand representation of the right M1 of rTMS at 1 Hz for 15 minutes at an intensity of 115% of the individual resting motor threshold.154 After the application of subthreshold rTMS at 1 Hz, patients with dystonia showed a significant reduction in mean writing pressure that was associated with clear but transient improvement.155
We have offered support for TMS as a potential therapeutic tool through the promotion of beneficial plasticity in the human brain. In some patients, rTMS can reinforce deficient neuronal pathways and may improve behavior temporarily. The effects of TMS can be produced not only at the site of stimulation but also at distant connected sites, a finding that could have potential implications for therapeutic use in patients with Parkinson disease. Next, we speculate on the future uses of TMS.
Future Study and Influence of TMS on Human Brain Plasticity
The belief that plasticity occurs in the CNS and can contribute to the recovery process has found considerable support. Transcranial magnetic stimulation has been used to measure plastic changes in the CNS and to assess the efficacy of physical therapy strategies after stroke. Furthermore, rTMS is capable of producing long-lasting alterations in cortical properties. However, despite all of these applications, TMS currently is not used directly in rehabilitative therapy.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation can be used to map the cortex and assess its excitability and resultant changes following interventions in many patient populations. Changes in map size correlate with improved recovery in patients with stroke.108 Other investigators156 have used TMS to assess cortical plasticity and function in people with incomplete tetraplegia. Transcranial magnetic stimulation has been used to assess changes in inhibitory and excitatory activities in the motor cortex in patients with stroke and to evaluate whether these changes are related to the extent of a patient's recovery of function.157 The ability to accurately assess the physiological mechanisms of recovery with TMS will provide rehabilitation therapists with an opportunity to generate interventions tailored to the specific physiology of an individual patient.
The potential uses of rTMS as a therapeutic tool include producing effects on the cerebral cortex that outlast the stimulus. There is a need to define clearly the stimulation parameters (such as frequency, duration, and interpulse interval) for specific brain regions and specific patient populations before rTMS can be used safely in clinics.
The excitatory changes mentioned above last only minutes at the longest. Therefore, although these phenomena may represent precursors of LTD and LTP, they may result in less durable changes. Studies exploring the combination of TMS and dopaminergic agents in an effort to enhance synaptic plasticity and improve function in patients with chronic stroke are under way.
Especially exciting for therapists is the combination of TMS and physical therapy interventions. Several studies have demonstrated that rTMS is capable of improving symptoms temporarily in a variety of neurological disorders, including movement disorders, depression, epilepsy, stroke and, more recently, chronic pain conditions.151,155,158–160 However, the effects are unreliable, modest, and short-lived. Perhaps one aim of the therapeutic application of TMS should be to help the brain reach a state in which it learns better. Once an optimal state of learning is reached, interventions can proceed. This strategy may allow physical therapy interventions to be more efficient.
In summary, understanding of the basic properties of TMS and its application to therapeutics is still elementary and currently provides only suggestions. The possibility for implementation by physical therapists appears to warrant further exploration.
Footnotes
-
For more about this PTJ Special Series on the role of neuroimaging in rehabilitation, read the editorial by Richard K Shields on page 639.
Both authors provided concept/idea/research design, writing, data collection and analysis, subjects, project management, and fund procurement.
This work was presented as part of a platform series on neuroimaging of stroke rehabilitation at the Combined Sections Meeting of the American Physical Therapy Association; February 23–27, 2005; New Orleans, La.
- Received September 15, 2006.
- Accepted January 10, 2007.
- Physical Therapy